
Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society
SCIE-ISI SCOPUS (1995-2023)
1355-6177
1469-7661
Anh Quốc
Cơ quản chủ quản: CAMBRIDGE UNIV PRESS , Cambridge University Press
Các bài báo tiêu biểu
The idea of reserve against brain damage stems from the repeated
observation that there does not appear to be a direct relationship
between the degree of brain pathology or brain damage and the
clinical manifestation of that damage. This paper attempts to
develop a coherent theoretical account of reserve. One convenient
subdivision of reserve models revolves around whether they envision
reserve as a passive process, such as in brain reserve or
threshold, or see the brain as actively attempting to cope with
or compensate for pathology, as in cognitive reserve. Cognitive
reserve may be based on more efficient utilization of brain
networks or of enhanced ability to recruit alternate brain networks
as needed. A distinction is suggested between reserve, the ability
to optimize or maximize normal performance, and compensation,
an attempt to maximize performance in the face of brain damage
by using brain structures or networks not engaged when the brain
is not damaged. Epidemiologic and imaging data that help to
develop and support the concept of reserve are presented.
(
This article considers the scientific process whereby new and better clinical tests of executive function might be developed, and what form they might take. We argue that many of the traditional tests of executive function most commonly in use (e.g., the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test; Stroop) are adaptations of procedures that emerged almost coincidentally from conceptual and experimental frameworks far removed from those currently in favour, and that the prolongation of their use has been encouraged by a sustained period of concentration on “construct-driven” experimentation in neuropsychology. This resulted from the special theoretical demands made by the field of executive function, but was not a necessary consequence, and may not even have been a useful one. Whilst useful, these tests may not therefore be optimal for their purpose. We consider as an alternative approach a function-led development programme which in principle could yield tasks better suited to the concerns of the clinician because of the transparency afforded by increased “representativeness” and “generalisability.” We further argue that the requirement of such a programme to represent the interaction between the individual and situational context might also provide useful constraints for purely experimental investigations. We provide an example of such a programme with reference to the Multiple Errands and Six Element tests. (
Performance on many cognitive and neuropsychological tests
may be improved by prior exposure to testing stimuli and
procedures. These beneficial practice effects can have a
significant impact on test performance when conventional
neuropsychological tests are administered at test–retest
intervals of weeks, months or years. Many recent investigations
have sought to determine changes in cognitive function over
periods of minutes or hours (e.g., before and after anesthesia)
using computerized tests. However, the effects of practice at
such brief test–retest intervals has not been reported.
The current study sought to determine the magnitude of practice
effects in a group of 113 individuals assessed with an automated
cognitive test battery on 4 occasions in 1 day. Practice effects
were evident both between and within assessments, and also within
individual tests. However, these effects occurred mostly between
the 1st and 2nd administration of the test battery, with smaller,
nonsignificant improvements observed between the 2nd, 3rd, and
4th administrations. On the basis of these results, methodological
and statistical strategies that may aid in the differentiation
of practice effects from drug-induced cognitive changes are
proposed. (
The possible medicinal use of cannabinoids for chronic diseases
emphasizes the need to understand the long-term effects of these
compounds on the central nervous system. We provide a quantitative
synthesis of empirical research pertaining to the non-acute
(residual) effects of cannabis on the neurocognitive performance
of adult human subjects. Out of 1,014 studies retrieved using
a thorough search strategy, only 11 studies met essential
A meta-analysis of 68 studies with a total of 4644 participants was conducted to investigate the sensitivity of tests of verbal fluency to the presence of Parkinson's disease (PD) relative to healthy controls. Both phonemic and semantic fluency were moderately impaired but neither deficit qualified as a differential deficit relative to verbal intelligence or psychomotor speed. However, PD patients were significantly more impaired on semantic relative to phonemic fluency (
This article reports the outcome of a meta-analysis of the relation between the frontal lobes and memory as measured by tests of recognition, cued recall, and free recall. We reviewed experiments in which patients with documented, circumscribed frontal pathology were compared with normal control subjects on these three types of tests. Contrary to conventional wisdom, there is strong evidence that frontal damage disrupts performance on all three types of tests, with the greatest impairment in free recall, and the smallest in recognition.
This study describes psychometric properties of the NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery (NIHTB-CB) Composite Scores in an adult sample. The NIHTB-CB was designed for use in epidemiologic studies and clinical trials for ages 3 to 85. A total of 268 self-described healthy adults were recruited at four university-based sites, using stratified sampling guidelines to target demographic variability for age (20–85 years), gender, education, and ethnicity. The NIHTB-CB contains seven computer-based instruments assessing five cognitive sub-domains: Language, Executive Function, Episodic Memory, Processing Speed, and Working Memory. Participants completed the NIHTB-CB, corresponding gold standard validation measures selected to tap the same cognitive abilities, and sociodemographic questionnaires. Three Composite Scores were derived for both the NIHTB-CB and gold standard batteries: “Crystallized Cognition Composite,” “Fluid Cognition Composite,” and “Total Cognition Composite” scores. NIHTB Composite Scores showed acceptable internal consistency (Cronbach’s alphas=0.84 Crystallized, 0.83 Fluid, 0.77 Total), excellent test–retest reliability (r: 0.86–0.92), strong convergent (r: 0.78–0.90) and discriminant (r: 0.19–0.39) validities