The De-automatizing Function of Mindfulness Facets: an Empirical Test

Mindfulness - Tập 11 - Trang 940-952 - 2020
Giulia Fuochi1, Alberto Voci1
1Department FISPPA-Applied Psychology, University of Padova, Padua, Italy

Tóm tắt

Building upon the theoretical model of de-automatization, according to which the de-automatizing effect of mindfulness on cognitive processes has desirable health and social outcomes, we hypothesized that (1) dispositional mindfulness facets would be related to higher well-being (higher life satisfaction, lower perceived stress), and positive social outcomes (greater perspective taking, lower use of stereotypes); (2) these effects would be mediated by lower rumination, higher cognitive flexibility, higher decentering, and lower thought suppression, identified as de-automatization mediators. We tested this mediational model with SEM, in a large sample (N = 1153). Results supported the hypothesized model more for individual than for social outcomes. Cognitive flexibility, decentering, and to a smaller extent suppression mediated the relationships between mindfulness facets and the two well-being outcomes, while rumination was a mediator only in the model for perceived stress. Acting with awareness, nonjudging, and nonreactivity had mixed relationships with perspective taking: positive via cognitive flexibility, negative via rumination; rumination instead conveyed a positive association between observing and perspective taking. Cognitive flexibility also mediated the positive relationships between observing and describing and perspective taking. Acting with awareness, nonjudging, and nonreactivity were negatively related to stereotyping only via reduced suppression. Effects sizes (standardized indirect effects) were small to medium (from 0.02 to 0.20 in absolute value). The association between dispositional mindfulness and mental health indicators may be partially explained by a reduction in maladaptive automatized cognitive processes, while only some features of de-automatization positively relate to social outcomes.

Tài liệu tham khảo

Albiero, P., Ingoglia, S., & Lo Coco, A. (2006). A contribution to the Italian validation of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index. Testing, Psychometrics, Methodology in Applied Psychology, 13, 107–125. Alleva, J. M., Roelofs, J., Voncken, M. J., Meevissen, Y. M. C., & Alberts, H. J. E. M. (2014). On the relation between mindfulness and depressive symptoms: rumination as a possible mediator. Mindfulness, 5, 72–79. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-012-0153-y. Andersen, S. M., & Limpert, C. (2001). Future-event schemas: automaticity and rumination in major depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 25, 311–333. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1026447600924. Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., & Toney, L. (2006). Using self-report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment, 13, 27–45. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191105283504. Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (1999). The unbearable automaticity of being. American Psychologist, 54, 462–479. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.54.7.462. Bastian, B., & Haslam, N. (2006). Psychological essentialism and stereotype endorsement. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 228–235. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2005.03.003 Bentler, P. M., & Chou, C. P. (1987). Practical issues in structural modeling. Sociological Methods & Research, 16, 78–117. https://doi.org/10.1177/0049124187016001004. Bergomi, C., Tschacher, W., & Kupper, Z. (2013). The assessment of mindfulness with self-report measures: existing scales and open issues. Mindfulness, 4, 191–202. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-012-0110-9. Boyraz, G., & Waits, J. B. (2015). Reciprocal associations among self-focused attention, self-acceptance, and empathy: a two-wave panel study. Personality and Individual Differences, 74, 84–89. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.09.042. Brown, D. B., Bravo, A. J., Roos, C. R., & Pearson, M. R. (2015). Five facets of mindfulness and psychological symptoms: evaluating a psychological model of the mechanism of mindfulness. Mindfulness, 6, 1021–1032. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-014-0349-4. Coffey, K. A., Hartman, M., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2010). Deconstructing mindfulness and constructing mental health: understanding mindfulness and its mechanisms of action. Mindfulness, 1, 235–253. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-010-0033-2. Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., & Mermelstein, R. (1983). A global measure of perceived stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 24, 385–396. https://doi.org/10.2307/2136404. Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 113–126. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.44.1.113. Dekeyser, M., Raes, F., Leijssen, M., Leysen, S., & Dewulf, D. (2008). Mindfulness skills and interpersonal behaviour. Personality and Individual Differences, 44, 1235–1245. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2007.11.018. Diener, E. D., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 71–75. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13. Felsman, P., Verduyn, P., Ayduk, O., & Kross, E. (2017). Being present: focusing on the present predicts improvements in life satisfaction but not happiness. Emotion, 17, 1047–1051. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000333. Fresco, D. M., Moore, M. T., Van Dulmen, M. H. M., Segal, Z. V., Ma, S. H., Teasdale, J. D., & Williams, J. M. G. (2007). Initial psychometric properties of the experiences questionnaire: validation of a self-report measure of decentering. Behavior Therapy, 38, 234–246. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2006.08.003. Galla, B. M., & Duckworth, A. L. (2015). More than resisting temptation: beneficial habits mediate the relationship between self-control and positive life outcomes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109, 508–525. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000026. Gallant, S. N. (2016). Mindfulness meditation practice and executive functioning: breaking down the benefit. Consciousness and Cognition, 40, 116–130. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2016.01.005. Giovannini, C., Giromini, L., Bonalume, L., Tagini, A., Lang, M., & Amadei, G. (2014). The Italian five facet mindfulness questionnaire: a contribution to its validity and reliability. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 36, 415–423. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-013-9403-0. Hewstone, M., Rubin, M., & Willis, H. (2002). Intergroup bias. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 575–604. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135109 Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1–55. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118. Jackson, D. L. (2003). Revisiting sample size and number of parameter estimates: some support for the N:q hypothesis. Structural Equation Modeling, 10, 128–141. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15328007SEM1001_6. Kang, Y., Gruber, J., & Gray, J. R. (2013). Mindfulness and de-automatization. Emotion Review, 5, 192–201. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073912451629. Kline, R. B. (2010). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (3rd ed.). New York: Guilford Press. Lebois, L. A. M., Papies, E. K., Gopinath, K., Cabanban, R., Quigley, K. S., Krishnamurthy, V., Feldman Barrett, L., & Barsalou, L. W. (2015). A shift in perspective: decentering through mindful attention to imagined stressful events. Neuropsychologia, 75, 505–524. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.05.030. Little, T. D., Cunningham, W. A., Shahar, G., & Widaman, K. F. (2002). To parcel or not to parcel: Exploring the question, weighing the merits. Structural Equation Modeling, 9, 151–173. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15328007SEM0902_1 Lueke, A., & Gibson, B. (2015). Mindfulness meditation reduces implicit age and race bias. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 6, 284–291. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550614559651. Martin, M. M., & Rubin, R. B. (1995). A new measure of cognitive flexibility. Psychological Reports, 76, 623–626. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1995.76.2.623. Miočević, M., O’Rourke, H. P., MacKinnon, D. P., & Brown, H. C. (2018). Statistical properties of four effect-size measures for mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, 50, 285–301. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-017-0870-1. Mrazek, M. D., Franklin, M. S., Phillips, D. T., Baird, B., & Schooler, J. W. (2013). Mindfulness training improves working memory capacity and GRE performance while reducing mind wandering. Psychological Science, 24, 776–781. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612459659. Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2011). Mplus user’s guide. Los Angeles: Muthén & Muthén. Ostafin, B. D., Bauer, C., & Myxter, P. (2012). Mindfulness decouples the relation between automatic alcohol motivation and heavy drinking. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 31, 729–745. https://doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2012.31.7.729. Panek, E. T., Bayer, J. B., Dal Cin, S., & Campbell, S. W. (2015). Automaticity, mindfulness, and self-control as predictors of dangerous texting behavior. Mobile Media & Communication, 3, 383–400. https://doi.org/10.1177/2050157915576046. Papies, E. K., Barsalou, L. W., & Custers, R. (2012). Mindful attention prevents mindless impulses. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3, 291–299. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550611419031. Pearson, M. R., Brown, D. B., Bravo, A. J., & Witkiewitz, K. (2015). Staying in the moment and finding purpose: the associations of trait mindfulness, decentering, and purpose in life with depressive symptoms, anxiety symptoms, and alcohol-related problems. Mindfulness, 6, 645–653. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-014-0300-8. Peters, J. R., Smart, L. M., Eisenlohr-Moul, T. A., Geiger, P. J., Smith, G. T., & Baer, R. A. (2015). Anger rumination as a mediation of the relationship between mindfulness and aggression: the utility of a multidimensional mindfulness model. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 71, 871–884. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.22189. Pettigrew, T. F., & Tropp, L. R. (2006). A meta-analytic test of intergroup contact theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 751–783. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.90.5.751. Pica, G., Amato, C., Mauro, R., & Pierro, A. (2015). Individual differences in preference for thought suppression: components and correlates of the white bear suppression inventory. Testing, Psychometrics, Methodology in Applied Psychology, 22, 31–42. https://doi.org/10.4473/TPM22.1.3. Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 879–891. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.40.3.879. R Core Team. (2019). R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing https://www.R-project.org/. Rosseel, Y. (2012). Lavaan: an R package for structural equation modeling. Journal of Statistical Software, 48, 1–36. Safran, J. D., & Segal, Z. V. (1990). Interpersonal process in cognitive therapy. New York: Basic Books. Sahdra, B., Ciarrochi, J., & Parker, P. (2016). Nonattachment and mindfulness: related but distinct constructs. Psychological Assessment, 28, 819–829. https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0000264. Sassenberg, K., & Moskowitz, G. B. (2005). Don’t stereotype, think different! Overcoming automatic stereotype activation by mindset priming. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 506–514. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2004.10.002. Schneider, W., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: detection, search, and attention. Psychological Review, 84, 1–62. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.84.1.1. Shapiro, S. L., Carlson, L. E., Astin, J. A., & Freedman, B. (2006). Mechanisms of mindfulness. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 62, 373–386. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.20237. Short, M. M., Mazmanian, D., Oinonen, K., & Mushquash, C. J. (2016). Executive function and self-regulation mediate dispositional mindfulness and well-being. Personality and Individual Differences, 93, 97–103. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.08.007. Teasdale, J. D. (1999). Metacognition, mindfulness and the modification of mood disorders. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 6, 146–155. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-0879(199905)6:2<146::AID-CPP195>3.0.CO;2-E. Tiffany, S. T., & Conklin, C. A. (2000). A cognitive processing model of alcohol craving and compulsive alcohol use. Addiction, 95, 145–153. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1360-0443.95.8s2.3.x. Tincher, M. M., Lebois, L. A. M., & Barsalou, L. W. (2016). Mindful attention reduces linguistic intergroup bias. Mindfulness, 7, 349–360. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-015-0450-3. Trapnell, P. D., & Campbell, J. D. (1999). Private self-consciousness and the five-factor model of personality: distinguishing rumination from reflection. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 284–304. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.76.2.284. Vannucci, M., & Chiorri, C. (2018). Individual differences in self-consciousness and mind wandering: further evidence for a dissociation between spontaneous and deliberate mind wandering. Personality and Individual Differences, 121, 57–61. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.09.022. Wegner, D. M., & Zanakos, S. (1994). Chronic thought suppression. Journal of Personality, 62, 615–640. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1994.tb00311.x. Whitehead, R., Bates, G., Elphinstone, B., Yang, Y., & Murray, G. (2019). Nonattachment mediates the relationship between mindfulness and psychological well-being, subjective well-being, and depression, anxiety and stress. Journal of Happiness Studies, 20, 2141-2158. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-018-0041-9. Wolf, E. J., Harrington, K. M., Clark, S. L., & Miller, M. W. (2013). Sample size requirements for structural equation models: an evaluation of power, bias, and solution propriety. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 73, 913–934. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164413495237. Zhao, X., Lynch, J. G., & Chen, Q. (2010). Reconsidering Baron and Kenny: myths and truths about mediation analysis. Journal of Consumer Research, 37, 197–206. https://doi.org/10.1086/651257.