Individual differences in false belief understanding are stable from 3 to 6 years of age and predict children’s mental state talk with school friends

Journal of Experimental Child Psychology - Tập 108 - Trang 96-112 - 2011
Claire Hughes1, Rosie Ensor1, Alex Marks1
1Centre for Family Research, University of Cambridge, Free School Lane, Cambridge CB2 3RF, UK

Tài liệu tham khảo

Adrián, 2007, Mothers’ use of cognitive state verbs in picture-book reading and the development of children’s understanding of mind: A longitudinal study, Child Development, 78, 1052, 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01052.x Astington, 1999, A longitudinal study of the relation between language and theory-of-mind development, Developmental Psychology, 35, 1311, 10.1037/0012-1649.35.5.1311 Badenes, 2000, Theory of mind and peer rejection at school, Social Development, 9, 273, 10.1111/1467-9507.00125 Balas, V. (2003). Drôles de plats! Paris: Nathan. Banerjee, 2002, Children’s understanding of self-presentational behavior: Links with mental-state reasoning and the attribution of embarrassment, Merrill–Palmer Quarterly, 48, 378, 10.1353/mpq.2002.0015 Bartsch, 1989, Young children’s attribution of action to beliefs and desires, Child Development, 60, 946, 10.2307/1131035 Bartsch, 1995 Bernard, 2007, Conversational perspective-taking and false belief attribution: A longitudinal study, British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 25, 443, 10.1348/026151006X171451 Bosacki, 2000, Theory of mind and self-concept in preadolescents: Links with gender and language, Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 709, 10.1037/0022-0663.92.4.709 Bosacki, 2004, Preschoolers’ understanding of simple and complex emotions: Links with gender and language, Sex Roles, 50, 659, 10.1023/B:SERS.0000027568.26966.27 Bretherton, 1982, Talking about internal states: The acquisition of a theory of mind, Developmental Psychology, 18, 906, 10.1037/0012-1649.18.6.906 Bright-Paul, 2008, Theory-of-mind development influences suggestibility and source monitoring, Developmental Psychology, 44, 1055, 10.1037/0012-1649.44.4.1055 Brown, 2006 Brown, 1996, Why talk about mental states? The significance of children’s conversations with friends, siblings, and mothers, Child Development, 67, 836, 10.2307/1131864 Carlson, 2004, Executive function and theory of mind: Stability and prediction from ages 2 to 3, Developmental Psychology, 40, 1105, 10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.1105 Charman, 1997, Reliability of theory of mind task performance by individuals with a learning disability: A research note, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 725, 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01699.x Charman, 2002, Is there a gender difference in false belief development?, Social Development, 11, 1, 10.1111/1467-9507.00183 Colvert, 2008, Emotional difficulties in early adolescence following severe early deprivation: Findings from the English and Romanian Adoptees Study, Development and Psychopathology, 20, 547, 10.1017/S0954579408000278 Cutting, 2006, Conversations with siblings and with friends: Links between relationship quality and social understanding, British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24, 73, 10.1348/026151005X70337 Davidson, 2001, Externalisms, 1 De Villiers, 2002, Complements to cognition: A longitudinal study of the relationship between complex syntax and false-belief-understanding, Cognitive Development, 17, 1037, 10.1016/S0885-2014(02)00073-4 DeFries, 1987, Genetic stability of cognitive development from childhood to adulthood, Developmental Psychology, 23, 4, 10.1037/0012-1649.23.1.4 Dunn, 1997 Dunn, 1991, Young children’s understanding of other people’s feelings and beliefs: Individual differences and their antecedents, Child Development, 62, 1352, 10.2307/1130811 Dunn, 1999, Understanding others, and individual differences in friendship interactions in young children, Social Development, 8, 201, 10.1111/1467-9507.00091 Dunn, 2001, “I got some swords and you’re dead!” Violent fantasy, antisocial behavior, friendship, and moral sensibility in young “hard to manage” children, Child Development, 72, 491, 10.1111/1467-8624.00292 Elliott, 1983 Embretson, 1996, The new rules of measurement, Psychological Assessment, 8, 341, 10.1037/1040-3590.8.4.341 Ensor, 2008, Content or connectedness? Early family talk and theory of mind in the toddler and preschool years, Child Development, 79, 201, 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01120.x Fabricius, 2003, False beliefs or false positives? Limits on children’s understanding of mental representation, Journal of Cognition and Development, 4, 239, 10.1207/S15327647JCD0403_01 Flynn, 2006, A microgenetic investigation of stability and continuity in theory of mind development, British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24, 631, 10.1348/026151005X57422 Flynn, 2007, The role of inhibitory control in false-belief understanding, Infant and Child Development, 16, 53, 10.1002/icd.500 Flynn, 2004, A longitudinal, microgenetic study of the emergence of false-belief understanding and inhibition skills, Developmental Science, 7, 103, 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00326.x Gerstadt, 1994, The relationship between cognition and action: Performance of children 3½–7 years old on a Stroop-like day–night test, Cognition, 53, 129, 10.1016/0010-0277(94)90068-X Gopnik, 1988, Children’s understanding of representational change and its relation to the understanding of false belief and the appearance–reality distinction, Child Development, 59, 26, 10.2307/1130386 Gorsuch, 1997, Exploratory factor analysis: Its role in item analysis, Journal of Personality Assessment, 68, 532, 10.1207/s15327752jpa6803_5 Happé, 1995, The role of age and verbal ability in the theory of mind task performance of subjects with autism, Child Development, 66, 843, 10.2307/1131954 Harris, 1989, Young children’s theory of mind and emotion, Cognition and Emotion, 3, 379, 10.1080/02699938908412713 Hughes, 1998, Finding your marbles: Does preschoolers’ strategic behavior predict later understanding of mind?, Developmental Psychology, 34, 1326, 10.1037/0012-1649.34.6.1326 Hughes, 2000, Good test–retest reliability for standard and advanced false-belief tasks across a wide range of abilities, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 483, 10.1111/1469-7610.00633 Hughes, 1999, Nature, nurture, and individual differences in early understanding of mind, Psychological Science, 10, 429, 10.1111/1467-9280.00181 Hughes, 2001, Acting nasty in the face of failure? Longitudinal observations of “hard to manage” children playing a rigged competitive game with a friend, Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 29, 403, 10.1023/A:1010495319182 Hughes, 1997, “Pretend you didn’t know”: Preschoolers’ talk about mental states in pretend play, Cognitive Development, 12, 477, 10.1016/S0885-2014(97)90019-8 Hughes, 1998, Understanding mind and emotion: Longitudinal associations with mental-state talk between young friends, Developmental Psychology, 34, 1026, 10.1037/0012-1649.34.5.1026 Hughes, 2002, “When I say a naughty word”: Children’s accounts of anger and sadness in self, mother, and friend—Longitudinal findings from ages four to seven, British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 20, 515, 10.1348/026151002760390837 Hughes, 2005, Theory of mind and executive function in 2-year-olds: A family affair?, Developmental Neuropsychology, 28, 645, 10.1207/s15326942dn2802_5 Hughes, 2007, Executive function and theory of mind: Predictive relations from ages 2- to 4-years, Developmental Psychology, 43, 1447, 10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1447 Hughes, 2006, Cooperation and conversations about the mind: A study of individual differences in two-year-olds and their siblings, British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24, 53, 10.1348/026151005X82893 Hughes, 2005, Origins of individual differences in theory of mind: From nature to nurture?, Child Development, 76, 356, 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00850_a.x Hughes, 2004, What are the links between theory of mind and social relations? Review, reflections, and new directions for studies of typical and atypical development, Social Development, 13, 590, 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2004.00285.x Hughes, 2000, Antisocial, angry, and unsympathetic: “Hard to manage” preschoolers’ peer problems and possible social and cognitive influences, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 169, 10.1017/S0021963099005193 Jenkins, 2000, Theory of mind and social behavior: Causal models tested in a longitudinal study, Merrill–Palmer Quarterly, 46, 203 Kenny, 2006 Ladd, 1997, Classroom peer acceptance, friendship, and victimization: Distinct relational systems that contribute uniquely to children’s school adjustment, Child Development, 68, 1181, 10.2307/1132300 Liu, 2008, Theory of mind development in Chinese children: A meta-analysis of false-belief understanding across cultures and languages, Developmental Psychology, 44, 523, 10.1037/0012-1649.44.2.523 Lockl, 2007, Knowledge about the mind: Links between theory of mind and later metamemory, Child Development, 78, 148, 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.00990.x Lockl, 2004, Sprache und theory of mind: Eine längsschnittstudie bei drei-bis vierjährigen [Language and theory of mind: A longitudinal study of three- to four-year-olds], Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Padagogische Psychologie, 36, 207, 10.1026/0049-8637.36.4.207 Lu, 2008, Talking about others facilitates theory of mind in Chinese preschoolers, Developmental Psychology, 44, 1726, 10.1037/a0013074 MacWhinney, 1985, The child language data exchange system, Journal of Child Language, 12, 271, 10.1017/S0305000900006449 Mayes, 1996, Test–retest reliability for false-belief tasks, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 313, 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1996.tb01408.x McAlister, 2007, A longitudinal study of child siblings and theory of mind development, Cognitive Development, 22, 258, 10.1016/j.cogdev.2006.10.009 McElwain, 2002, Relating individual control, social understanding, and gender to child–friend interaction: A relationships perspective, Social Development, 11, 362, 10.1111/1467-9507.00205 Merrell, 1994 Miller, 2009, Children’s understanding of second-order mental states, Psychological Bulletin, 135, 749, 10.1037/a0016854 Milligan, 2007, Language and theory of mind: Meta-analysis of the relation between language ability and false-belief understanding, Child Development, 78, 622, 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01018.x Moerbeek, 1994 Moses, 2001, Executive accounts of theory of mind development, Child Development, 3, 688, 10.1111/1467-8624.00306 Onishi, 2005, Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs?, Science, 308, 255, 10.1126/science.1107621 Parker, 1993, Friendship and friendship quality in middle childhood: Links with peer group acceptance and feelings of loneliness and social dissatisfaction, Developmental Psychology, 29, 611, 10.1037/0012-1649.29.4.611 Perner, 2002, Theory of mind and self-control: More than a common problem of inhibition, Child Development, 73, 752, 10.1111/1467-8624.00436 Perner, 1985, “John thinks that Mary thinks that …”: Attribution of second-order beliefs by 5- to 10-year-old children, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 39, 437, 10.1016/0022-0965(85)90051-7 Peterson, 1995, Deafness, conversation, and theory of mind, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36, 459, 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1995.tb01303.x Peterson, 2002, Mindreading and moral awareness in popular and rejected preschoolers, British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 20, 205, 10.1348/026151002166415 Razza, 2009, Associations among false-belief understanding, executive function, and social competence. A longitudinal analysis, Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 30, 332, 10.1016/j.appdev.2008.12.020 Ronald, 2005, Nice and nasty theory of mind: Genetic and environmental contributions, Social Development, 14, 664, 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2005.00323.x Ronald, 2006, Individual differences in theory of mind ability in middle childhood and links with verbal ability and autistic traits: A twin study, Social Neuroscience, 1, 412, 10.1080/17470910601068088 Russell, 2007, Sex differences in theory of mind: A male advantage on Happé’s “cartoon” task, Cognition and Emotion, 21, 1554, 10.1080/02699930601117096 Serra, 2002, ). Theory of mind in children with “lesser variants” of autism: A longitudinal study, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 43, 885, 10.1111/1469-7610.00104 Shatz, 1973, The development of communication skills: Modifications in the speech of young children as a function of listener, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 38, 1, 10.2307/1165783 Slade, 2005, How language does (and does not) relate to theory of mind: A longitudinal study of syntax, semantics, working memory, and false belief, British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23, 117, 10.1348/026151004X21332 Slaughter, 2002, Theory of mind and peer acceptance in preschool children, British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 20, 545, 10.1348/026151002760390945 Slomkowski, 1996, Young children’s understanding of other people’s beliefs and feelings and their connected communication with friends, Developmental Psychology, 32, 442, 10.1037/0012-1649.32.3.442 Sullivan, 1994, Preschoolers can attribute second-order beliefs, Developmental Psychology, 30, 395, 10.1037/0012-1649.30.3.395 Wellman, 2001, Meta-analysis of theory of mind development: The truth about false belief, Child Development, 72, 655, 10.1111/1467-8624.00304 Wellman, 2004, Scaling of theory-of-mind tasks, Child Development, 75, 523, 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00691.x Wimmer, 1983, Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception, Cognition, 13, 103, 10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5 Youngblade, 1995, Individual differences in young children’s pretend play with mother and sibling: Links to relationships and understanding of other people’s feelings and beliefs, Child Development, 66, 1472, 10.2307/1131658