Ferrier lecture - Functional architecture of macaque monkey visual cortex

The Royal Society - Tập 198 Số 1130 - Trang 1-59 - 1977
David H. Hubel1, Torsten N. Wiesel1
1Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts 02115, U.S.A.

Tóm tắt

Of the many possible functions of the macaque monkey primary visual cortex (striate cortex, area 17) two are now fairly well understood. First, the incoming information from the lateral geniculate bodies is rearranged so that most cells in the striate cortex respond to specifically oriented line segments, and, second, information originating from the two eyes converges upon single cells. The rearrangement and convergence do not take place immediately, however: in layer IVc, where the bulk of the afferents terminate, virtually all cells have fields with circular symmetry and are strictly monocular, driven from the left eye or from the right, but not both; at subsequent stages, in layers above and below IVc, most cells show orientation specificity, and about half are binocular. In a binocular cell the receptive fields in the two eyes are on corresponding regions in the two retinas and are identical in structure, but one eye is usually more effective than the other in influencing the cell; all shades of ocular dominance are seen. These two functions are strongly reflected in the architecture of the cortex, in that cells with common physiological properties are grouped together in vertically organized systems of columns. In an ocular dominance column all cells respond preferentially to the same eye. By four independent anatomical methods it has been shown that these columns have the form of vertically disposed alternating left-eye and right-eye slabs, which in horizontal section form alternating stripes about 400 μm thick, with occasional bifurcations and blind endings. Cells of like orientation specificity are known from physiological recordings to be similarly grouped in much narrower vertical sheeet-like aggregations, stacked in orderly sequences so that on traversing the cortex tangentially one normally encounters a succession of small shifts in orientation, clockwise or counterclockwise; a 1 mm traverse is usually accompanied by one or several full rotations through 180°, broken at times by reversals in direction of rotation and occasionally by large abrupt shifts. A full complement of columns, of either type, left-plus-right eye or a complete 180° sequence, is termed a hypercolumn. Columns (and hence hypercolumns) have roughly the same width throughout the binocular part of the cortex. The two independent systems of hypercolumns are engrafted upon the well known topographic representation of the visual field. The receptive fields mapped in a vertical penetration through cortex show a scatter in position roughly equal to the average size of the fields themselves, and the area thus covered, the aggregate receptive field, increases with distance from the fovea. A parallel increase is seen in reciprocal magnification (the number of degrees of visual field corresponding to 1 mm of cortex). Over most or all of the striate cortex a movement of 1-2 mm, traversing several hypercolumns, is accompanied by a movement through the visual field about equal in size to the local aggregate receptive field. Thus any 1-2 mm block of cortex contains roughly the machinery needed to subserve an aggregate receptive field. In the cortex the fall-off in detail with which the visual field is analysed, as one moves out from the foveal area, is accompanied not by a reduction in thickness of layers, as is found in the retina, but by a reduction in the area of cortex (and hence the number of columnar units) devoted to a given amount of visual field: unlike the retina, the striate cortex is virtually uniform morphologically but varies in magnification. In most respects the above description fits the newborn monkey just as well as the adult, suggesting that area 17 is largely genetically programmed. The ocular dominance columns, however, are not fully developed at birth, since the geniculate terminals belonging to one eye occupy layer IVc throughout its length, segregating out into separate columns only after about the first 6 weeks, whether or not the animal has visual experience. If one eye is sutured closed during this early period the columns belonging to that eye become shrunken and their companions correspondingly expanded. This would seem to be at least in part the result of interference with normal maturation, though sprouting and retraction of axon terminals are not excluded.

Từ khóa


Tài liệu tham khảo

10.1007/BF00234062

10.1113/jphysiol.1964.sp007463

10.1113/jphysiol.1962.sp006991

Bishop P. O., 1972, Striate neurons: receptive field concepts, Investigative Ophthalmol., 11, 346

10.1007/BF00234129

Blakemore C., 1975, Innate and environmental factors in the development of the kitten's visual cortex. J . Physiol, Lond., 248, 663

10.1093/brain/49.1.1

10.1016/0006-8993(75)90749-0

Clark W. E., 1941, The laminar organization and cell content of the lateral geniculate body in the monkey. J, Anat. (Lond), 75, 419

Clark W. E., 1939, Structural changes in the isolated visual cortex, J.Anat. (Lond), 73, 563

Cleland B. G., 1974, Properties of rarely encountered types of ganglion cells in the cat's retina and an overall classification. J . Physiol, Lond., 240, 457

10.1016/0006-8993(72)90344-7

Daniel P. M., 1961, The representation of the visual field on the cerebral cortex in monkeys. J . Physiol, Lond., 159, 203

10.1016/0006-8993(74)90607-6

10.1002/cne.901600302

10.1016/0006-8993(67)90166-7

10.1016/0006-8993(73)90782-8

10.1002/cne.901630106

Gilbert C. D. 1977 Laminar differences in receptive field properties of cells in cat primary visual cortex. J . Physiol. Land. (In the press.)

Glendenning K. K. & Kofron E. A. 1974 Projections of individual laminae of the lateral geniculate nucleus in the prosimian. 4 thann. meet. Soc. Neurosci. (St Louis) Abstr. p. 229.

10.1002/ar.1091580313

10.1016/0014-4886(73)90040-X

10.1002/cne.901280104

10.1002/cne.901440106

10.1007/BF00335403

10.1002/cne.901390403

Hartline H. K., 1940, The receptive fields of optic nerve fibers. Amer. J, Physiol., 130, 690

10.1152/jn.1972.35.4.518

10.1016/0006-8993(75)90568-5

10.1016/0006-8993(77)90299-2

Hubei D. H., 1959, Receptive fields of single neurones in the cat's striate cortex. J . Physiol, Lond., 148, 574

Hubei D. H. & Wiesel T. N. i960 Receptive fields of optic nerve fibres in the spider monkey. J . Physiol. Lond. 154 572-580.

Hubei D. H., 1961, Integrative action in the cat's lateral geniculate body. J . Physiol, Lond., 155, 385

Hubei D. H., 1962, Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex. J . Physiol, Lond., 160, 106

10.1152/jn.1963.26.6.994

10.1152/jn.1965.28.2.229

Hubei D. H., 1968, Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex. J . Physiol, Lond., 195, 215

10.1038/225041a0

10.1002/cne.901460402

Hubei D. H., 1973, A re-examination of stereoscopic mechanisms in area 17 of the cat. J . Physiol, Lond., 232, 29

10.1002/cne.901580304

10.1002/cne.901580305

Hubei D. H. Wiesel T. N. & LeVay S. 1974 Visual-field representation in layer IV C of monkey striate cortex. 4 t h a n n . meet. Soc.(St Louis) Abstr. p. 264.

Hubei D. H., 1976, Functional architecture of area 17 in normal and monocularly deprived macaque monkeys, Gold Spr. Harbor Quant. Biol., 40, 681

Hubei D. H., 1977, Plasticity of ocular dominance columns in monkey striate cortex, Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Loud. B, 278, 131

Kelly J. P., 1974, Cell structure and function in the visual cortex of the cat. J .Physiol, Loud., 238, 515

Kennedy C., 1975, The ocular dominance columns of the striate cortex as studied by the deoxyglucose method for measurement of local cerebral glucose utilization, Trans. Am. Neurol. Ass., 100, 74

10.1152/jn.1953.16.1.37

10.1016/0006-8993(68)90052-8

10.1002/cne.901590408

Lettvin J. Y. Maturana H. R. McCulloch W. S. & Pitts W. H. 1959 What the frog's eye tells the frog's brain. Proc. Inst. Radio Engrs. N .Y . 47 1940-1951.

10.1002/cne.901470404

10.1002/cne.901640303

Minkowski M., 1920, 2. Uber den Verlauf, die Endigung und die zentrale Representation von gekreuzten und ungekreuzten Sehnervenfasem bei einigen Saugetieren und beim Menschen, Schweiz Arch. Neurol. Psychiat., 6, 201

10.1152/jn.1957.20.4.408

10.1016/0006-8993(74)90295-9

Polyak S. 1957 The vertebrate visual system (ed. H. Kliiver) Chicago 111.: University of Chicago Press.

Portmann A. 1948 Tiergestalt - Basel: Reinhardt. Studieniiber die Bedeutung der tierischen Erscheinung.

10.1098/rstb.1977.0040

10.1016/0006-8993(75)90322-4

10.1016/0042-6989(69)90117-5

10.1152/jn.1976.39.6.1288

Shatz C. J. Lindstrom S. H. &Wiesel T. N. 1977 The distribution of afferents representing the right and left eyes in the cat's visual cortex. Brain Res. (In the press.)

Sherman S. M. 1973 Visual field defects in monocularly and binocularly deprived cats Brain Res. 49 25-45.

10.1002/cne.901580102

Sokoloff L. 1975 Influence of functional activity on local cerebral glucose utilization. In Brain work. The coupling of function metabolism and blood flow in the brain (ed. D. H. Ingvar and N. A. Lassen) pp. 385-388. New York: Academic Press.

Sokoloff L. 1977 Measurement of local glucose utilization and its use in mapping local functional activity in the central nervous system. Proc. 4 World Congress of Psychiatric Surgery Madrid Sept. 7-10 1975 pp. 111-121.

Stone J. & Fukuda Y. 1974 Properties of cat retinal ganglion cells: a comparison of W-cells with X- and Y-cells. J ■Neurophysiol. 37 722-748.

10.1016/S0002-9394(41)91363-6

10.1007/BF00234257

Van Buren J. M. 1963 The retinal ganglion cell layer. Springfield 111.: Charles C. Thomas.

Wiesel T. N. i960 Receptive fields of ganglion cells in the cat's retina. J . Physiol. Lond. 153 583-594.

10.1152/jn.1963.26.6.1003

10.1152/jn.1965.28.6.1029

10.1152/jn.1966.29.6.1115

10.1002/cne.901580306

10.1016/0006-8993(69)90136-X