Exploring the development of attentional set shifting in young children with a novel Intradimensional/Extradimensional shift task
Tài liệu tham khảo
Althaus, 2015, Categorization in infancy: Labeling induces a persisting focus on commonalities, Developmental Science, 19, 1
Apicella, 2020, Beyond WEIRD: A review of the last decade and a look ahead to the global laboratory of the future, Evolution and Human Behavior, 41, 319, 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2020.07.015
Baayen, 2008
Bardikoff, 2017, The differentiation of executive functioning across development: Insights from developmental cognitive neuroscience, 27
Barr, 2013, Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal, Journal of Memory and Language, 68, 255, 10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001
Bartoń, K. (2020). MuMIn: Multi-model inference. https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/MuMIn/index.html
Bates, 2015, Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4, Journal of Statistical Software, 67, 1, 10.18637/jss.v067.i01
Benitez, 2017, Sustained selective attention predicts flexible switching in preschoolers, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 156, 29, 10.1016/j.jecp.2016.11.004
Best, 2010, A developmental perspective on executive function, Child Development, 81, 1641, 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01499.x
Best, 2011, Relations between executive function and academic achievement from ages 5 to 17 in a large, representative national sample, Learning and Individual Differences, 21, 327, 10.1016/j.lindif.2011.01.007
Blair, 2007, Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten, Child Development, 78, 647, 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01019.x
Blakey, 2018, Not all distractions are the same: Investigating why preschoolers make distraction errors when switching, Child Development, 89, 609, 10.1111/cdev.12721
Blakey, 2016, Different executive functions support different kinds of cognitive flexibility: Evidence from 2-, 3-, and 4-year-olds, Child Development, 87, 513, 10.1111/cdev.12468
Brown, 2015, Attentional set-shifting across species, Vol. 28, 363
Brown, 2015, Behavioral flexibility: Attentional shifting, rule switching, and response reversal, 264
Bull, 2004, A comparison of performance on the Towers of London and Hanoi in young children, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 743, 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00268.x
Bull, 2008, Short-term memory, working memory, and executive functioning in preschoolers: Longitudinal predictors of mathematical achievement at age 7 years, Developmental Neuropsychology, 33, 205, 10.1080/87565640801982312
Burgess, 1997, Theory and methodology in executive function research, 81
Burgoyne, 2020, Attention control: A cornerstone of higher-order cognition, Current Directions in Psychological Science, 29, 624, 10.1177/0963721420969371
Buss, 1953, Rigidity as a function of reversal and non-reversal shifts in the learning of successive discriminations, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 45, 75, 10.1037/h0054027
Buss, 2019, Dimensional attention as a mechanism of executive function: Integrating flexibility, selectivity, and stability, Cognition, 192, 10.1016/j.cognition.2019.06.015
Buss, 2020, Not all labels develop equally: The role of labels in guiding attention to dimensions, Cognitive Development, 53, 10.1016/j.cogdev.2019.100843
Buss, A. T., & Spencer, J. P. (2014). The emergent executive: A dynamic field theory of the development of executive function. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 79(2, Serial No. 313).
Buss, 2018, Changes in frontal and posterior cortical activity underlie the early emergence of executive function, Developmental Science, 21, 10.1111/desc.12602
Caron, 1969, Discrimination shifts in three-year-olds as a function of dimensional salience, Developmental Psychology, 1, 333, 10.1037/h0027698
Chevalier, 2008, Cognitive flexibility in preschoolers: The role of representation activation and maintenance, Developmental Science, 11, 339, 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00679.x
Cragg, 2012, The processes underlying flexibility in childhood, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 209, 10.1080/17470210903204618
Deák, 2000, The growth of flexible problem solving: Preschool children use changing verbal cues to infer multiple word meanings, Journal of Cognition and Development, 1, 157, 10.1207/S15327647JCD010202
Diamond, 2013, Executive functions, Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135, 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750
Diamond, 2016, Why improving and assessing executive functions early in life is critical, 11
Diamond, 2005, Preschool children’s performance in task switching on the Dimensional Change Card Sort task: Separating the dimensions aids the ability to switch, Developmental Neuropsychology, 28, 689, 10.1207/s15326942dn2802_7
Dickerson, 1966, Performance of preschool children on three discrimination shifts, Psychonomic Science, 4, 417, 10.3758/BF03342368
Dickerson, 1970, Discrimination shift performance of kindergarten children as a function of variation of the irrelevant shift dimension, Developmental Psychology, 3, 229, 10.1037/h0029576
Dobson, 2002
Doebel, 2015, A meta-analysis of the Dimensional Change Card Sort: Implications for developmental theories and the measurement of executive function in children, Developmental Review, 38, 241, 10.1016/j.dr.2015.09.001
Eckstrand, 1954, Transfer of perceptual set, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47, 274, 10.1037/h0062034
Eimas, 1965, Comment: Comparisons of reversal and nonreversal shifts, Psychonomic Science, 3, 445, 10.3758/BF03343223
Eimas, 1966, Effects of overtraining and age on intradimensional and extradimensional shifts in children, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 3, 348, 10.1016/0022-0965(66)90079-8
Esposito, 1975, Review of discrimination shift learning in young children, Psychological Bulletin, 82, 432, 10.1037/0033-2909.82.3.432
Espy, 1997, The shape school: Assessing executive function in preschool children, Developmental Neuropsychology, 13, 495, 10.1080/87565649709540690
Espy, 2004, The development of a trail making test in young children: The TRAILS-P, The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 18, 411, 10.1080/138540409052416
Field, 2005
Fisher, 2019, Selective sustained attention: A developmental foundation for cognition, Current Opinion in Psychology, 29, 248, 10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.06.002
FitzGibbon, 2014, Primed to be inflexible: The influence of set size on cognitive flexibility during childhood, Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00101
Forstmeier, 2011, Cryptic multiple hypotheses testing in linear models: Overestimated effect sizes and the winner’s curse, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 65, 47, 10.1007/s00265-010-1038-5
Frye, 1995, Theory of mind and rule-based reasoning, Cognitive Development, 10, 483, 10.1016/0885-2014(95)90024-1
Garon, 2008, Executive function in preschoolers: A review using an integrative framework, Psychological Bulletin, 134, 31, 10.1037/0033-2909.134.1.31
Hanania, 2010, Two types of perseveration in the Dimension Change Card Sort task, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 107, 325, 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.05.002
Hanania, 2010, Selective attention and attention switching: Towards a unified developmental approach, Developmental Science, 13, 622, 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00921.x
Henrich, 2010, The weirdest people in the world?, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 61, 10.1017/S0140525X0999152X
Henry, 2010, The assessment of executive functioning in children, Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 15, 110, 10.1111/j.1475-3588.2010.00557.x
Holding, 2018, Can we measure cognitive constructs consistently within and across cultures? Evidence from a test battery in Bangladesh, Ghana, and Tanzania, Applied Neuropsychology: Child, 7, 1, 10.1080/21622965.2016.1206823
Ionescu, 2012, Exploring the nature of cognitive flexibility, New Ideas in Psychology, 30, 190, 10.1016/j.newideapsych.2011.11.001
Karr, 2018, The unity and diversity of executive functions: A systematic review and re-analysis of latent variable studies, Psychological Bulletin, 144, 1147, 10.1037/bul0000160
Kendler, 1962, Vertical and horizontal processes in problem solving, Psychological Review, 69, 1, 10.1037/h0038537
Kendler, 1979, The development of discrimination learning: A levels-of-functioning explanation, Vol. 13, 83
Kendler, 1959, Reversal and nonreversal shifts in kindergarten children, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 56, 10.1037/h0041855
Kendler, 1960, Reversal and nonreversal shifts in nursery school children, Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 53, 83, 10.1037/h0039068
Kirkham, 2003, Helping children apply their knowledge to their behavior on a dimension-switching task, Developmental Science, 6, 449, 10.1111/1467-7687.00300
Kloo, 2005, Disentangling dimensions in the Dimensional Change Card Sorting task, Developmental Science, 8, 44, 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00392.x
Landry, 2017, 3-year-olds’ perseveration on the DCCS explained: A meta-analysis, Journal of Cognition and Development, 18, 419, 10.1080/15248372.2017.1345910
Lawrence, 1949, Acquired distinctiveness of cues: I. Transfer between discriminations on the basis of familiarity with the stimulus, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 39, 770, 10.1037/h0058097
Legare, 2018, Cultural variation in cognitive flexibility reveals diversity in the development of executive functions, Scientific Reports, 8, 16326, 10.1038/s41598-018-34756-2
Luciana, 2002, Assessment of neuropsychological function through use of the Cambridge Neuropsychological Testing Automated Battery: Performance in 4- to 12-year-old children, Developmental Neuropsychology, 22, 595, 10.1207/S15326942DN2203_3
Matuschek, 2017, Balancing Type I error and power in linear mixed models, Journal of Memory and Language, 94, 305, 10.1016/j.jml.2017.01.001
Miyake, 2000, The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis, Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49, 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734
Mumbauer, 1967, Variables affecting the performance of preschool children in intradimensional, reversal, and extradimensional shifts, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 75, 180, 10.1037/h0024990
Munakata, 2003, Children’s perseveration: Attentional inertia and alternative accounts, Developmental Science, 6, 471, 10.1111/1467-7687.00302
Nieuwenhuis, 2012, influence.ME: Tools for detecting influential data in mixed effects models, The R Journal, 4, 38, 10.32614/RJ-2012-011
Perner, 2002, What causes 3-year-olds’ difficulty on the Dimensional Change Card Sorting task?, Infant and Child Development, 11, 93, 10.1002/icd.299
Perry, L., & Samuelson, L. (2013). The role of verbal labels in attention to dimensional similarity. In M. Knauff (Ed.), Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (Vol. 35, pp. 3217–3222). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
R Core Team, 2020
Roberts, 1988, The effects of intradimensional and extradimensional shifts on visual discrimination learning in humans and non-human primates, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40, 321
Schielzeth, 2009, Conclusions beyond support: Overconfident estimates in mixed models, Behavioral Ecology, 20, 416, 10.1093/beheco/arn145
Settlage, 1956, Perseverative interference in monkeys following bilateral removal of the prefrontal areas, Journal of General Psychology, 54, 255, 10.1080/00221309.1956.9920281
Shanab, 1979, Intradimensional and extradimensional shifts by Jordanian students, Journal of General Psychology, 100, 199, 10.1080/00221309.1979.9710951
Shepp, 1971, Some effects of variable-within and variable-between irrelevant stimuli on dimensional learning and transfer, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 89, 32, 10.1037/h0031212
Sirois, 1998, Neural network modeling of developmental effects in discrimination shifts, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 71, 235, 10.1006/jecp.1998.2474
Slamecka, 1968, A methodological analysis of shift paradigms in human discrimination learning, Psychological Bulletin, 69, 423, 10.1037/h0025762
Smidts, 2004, The object classification task for children: A new measure of concept generation and mental flexibility in early childhood, Developmental Neuropsychology, 26, 385, 10.1207/s15326942dn2601_2
Smith, 1977, Developmental trends in free classification: Evidence for a new conceptualization of perceptual development, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 24, 279, 10.1016/0022-0965(77)90007-8
St Clair-Thompson, 2006, Executive functions and achievements in school: Shifting, updating, inhibition, and working memory, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 745, 10.1080/17470210500162854
Sugimura, 1970, Consecutive intradimensional, extradimensional, and control shifts in kindergarten children, Japanese Psychological Research, 12, 36, 10.4992/psycholres1954.12.36
Tait, 2018, Assessment of intradimensional/extradimensional attentional set-shifting in rats, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 89, 72, 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.02.013
Tighe, 2014, Overtraining and discrimination shift behavior in children, Psychonomic Science, 2, 365, 10.3758/BF03343501
Tighe, 1971, Subproblem analysis of discrimination-shift learning, Psychonomic Science, 24, 159, 10.3758/BF03335550
Völter, 2017, Causal and inferential reasoning in animals, 643
Völter, 2018, Comparative psychometrics: Establishing what differs is central to understanding what evolves, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373, 20170283, 10.1098/rstb.2017.0283
Völter, 2022, The structure of executive functions in preschool children and chimpanzees, Scientific Reports, 12, 10.1038/s41598-022-08406-7
Wolff, 1967, Concept-shift and discrimination-reversal learning in humans, Psychological Bulletin, 68, 369, 10.1037/h0025189
Zelazo, 2006, The Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS): A method of assessing executive function in children, Nature Protocols, 1, 297, 10.1038/nprot.2006.46
Zelazo, 2003, The development of executive function in early childhood, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 68, 93, 10.1111/j.0037-976X.2003.00266.x