Does Language Shape Thought?: Mandarin and English Speakers' Conceptions of Time
Tài liệu tham khảo
Boroditsky, 1998, Evidence for metaphoric representation: Understanding time
Boroditsky, 2000, Metaphoric structuring: Understanding time through spatial metaphors, Cognition, 75, 1, 10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00073-6
Boroditsky, L. (in preparation), Which way to the present? Cross-linguistic differences in thinking about time.
Bowerman, 1996, The origins of children's spatial semantic categories: cognitive versus linguistic determinants, 145
Choi, 1991, Learning to express motion events in English and Korean: The influence of language-specific lexicalization patterns. Special Issue: Lexical and conceptual semantics, Cognition, 41, 83, 10.1016/0010-0277(91)90033-Z
Choi, 1995, Early acquisition of verbs in Korean: A cross-linguistic study, Journal of Child Language, 22, 497, 10.1017/S0305000900009934
Chun, 1997, A cognitive approach to UP metaphors in English and Chinese: What do they reveal about the English mind and the Chinese mind?
Chun, 1997, Conceptualizing the world through spatial metaphors: An analysis of UP/DOWN vs. SHANG/XIA metaphor
Clark, 1973, Space, time semantics, and the child
Gelman, 1998, Acquisition of nouns and verbs in Mandarin and English
Gentner, 1982, Why nouns are learned before verbs: Linguistic relativity versus natural partitioning, 301
Gentner, 2001, Individuation, relational relativity and early word learning
Gentner, D. &, Boronat, C. 1992, May, Metaphor as mapping. Paper presented at the Workshop on Metaphor, Tel Aviv, Israel.
Gentner, 2001, Metaphor is like analogy
Gopnik, 1995, Names, relational words, and cognitive development in English and Korean Speakers: Nouns are not always learned before verbs, 63
Heider, 1972, Universals in color naming and memory, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 93, 10, 10.1037/h0032606
Hunt, 1991, The Whorfian hypothesis: A cognitive psychology perspective, Psychological Review, 98, 377, 10.1037/0033-295X.98.3.377
Johnson, 1989, Critical period effects in second language learning: The influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language, Cognitive Psychology, 21, 60, 10.1016/0010-0285(89)90003-0
Kay, 1984, What is the Sapir–Whorf hypothesis?, American Anthropologist, 86, 65, 10.1525/aa.1984.86.1.02a00050
Lehrer, 1990, Polysemy, conventionality, and the structure of the lexicon, Cognitive Linguistics, 1, 207, 10.1515/cogl.1990.1.2.207
Lucy, 1992
Lucy, 1979, Whorf and his critics: Linguistic and nonlinguistic influences on color memory, American Anthropologist, 81, 581, 10.1525/aa.1979.81.3.02a00040
Macnamara, 1972, Cognitive basis of language learning in infants, Psychological Review, 79, 1, 10.1037/h0031901
Rosch, 1975, Cognitive representations of semantic categories, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104, 192, 10.1037/0096-3445.104.3.192
Scott, 1989, The vertical dimension and time in Mandarin, Australian Journal of Linguistics, 9, 295, 10.1080/07268608908599424
Slobin, 1987, Thinking for speaking, Proceedings of the Berkeley Linguistic Society, 13, 435, 10.3765/bls.v13i0.1826
Tardif, 1996, Nouns are not always learned before verbs, but why? Evidence from Mandarin Chinese, Developmental Psychology, 32, 492, 10.1037/0012-1649.32.3.492
Traugott, 1978, On the expression of spatiotemporal relations in language, 369
Waxman, 1990, Nouns mark category relations: Toddlers' and Preschoolers' word-learning biases, Child Development, 61, 1461, 10.2307/1130756
Whorf, 1956