Does Language Shape Thought?: Mandarin and English Speakers' Conceptions of Time

Cognitive Psychology - Tập 43 - Trang 1-22 - 2001
Lera Boroditsky1
1Stanford University

Tài liệu tham khảo

Boroditsky, 1998, Evidence for metaphoric representation: Understanding time

Boroditsky, 2000, Metaphoric structuring: Understanding time through spatial metaphors, Cognition, 75, 1, 10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00073-6

Boroditsky, L. (in preparation), Which way to the present? Cross-linguistic differences in thinking about time.

Bowerman, 1996, The origins of children's spatial semantic categories: cognitive versus linguistic determinants, 145

Choi, 1991, Learning to express motion events in English and Korean: The influence of language-specific lexicalization patterns. Special Issue: Lexical and conceptual semantics, Cognition, 41, 83, 10.1016/0010-0277(91)90033-Z

Choi, 1995, Early acquisition of verbs in Korean: A cross-linguistic study, Journal of Child Language, 22, 497, 10.1017/S0305000900009934

Chun, 1997, A cognitive approach to UP metaphors in English and Chinese: What do they reveal about the English mind and the Chinese mind?

Chun, 1997, Conceptualizing the world through spatial metaphors: An analysis of UP/DOWN vs. SHANG/XIA metaphor

Clark, 1973, Space, time semantics, and the child

Gelman, 1998, Acquisition of nouns and verbs in Mandarin and English

Gentner, 1982, Why nouns are learned before verbs: Linguistic relativity versus natural partitioning, 301

Gentner, 2001, Individuation, relational relativity and early word learning

Gentner, D. &, Boronat, C. 1992, May, Metaphor as mapping. Paper presented at the Workshop on Metaphor, Tel Aviv, Israel.

Gentner, 2001, Metaphor is like analogy

Gillette, 1999, Human simulations of vocabulary learning, Cognition, 10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00036-0

Gopnik, 1995, Names, relational words, and cognitive development in English and Korean Speakers: Nouns are not always learned before verbs, 63

Heider, 1972, Universals in color naming and memory, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 93, 10, 10.1037/h0032606

Hunt, 1991, The Whorfian hypothesis: A cognitive psychology perspective, Psychological Review, 98, 377, 10.1037/0033-295X.98.3.377

Johnson, 1989, Critical period effects in second language learning: The influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language, Cognitive Psychology, 21, 60, 10.1016/0010-0285(89)90003-0

Kay, 1984, What is the Sapir–Whorf hypothesis?, American Anthropologist, 86, 65, 10.1525/aa.1984.86.1.02a00050

Lehrer, 1990, Polysemy, conventionality, and the structure of the lexicon, Cognitive Linguistics, 1, 207, 10.1515/cogl.1990.1.2.207

Lucy, 1992

Lucy, 1979, Whorf and his critics: Linguistic and nonlinguistic influences on color memory, American Anthropologist, 81, 581, 10.1525/aa.1979.81.3.02a00040

Macnamara, 1972, Cognitive basis of language learning in infants, Psychological Review, 79, 1, 10.1037/h0031901

Rosch, 1975, Cognitive representations of semantic categories, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104, 192, 10.1037/0096-3445.104.3.192

Scott, 1989, The vertical dimension and time in Mandarin, Australian Journal of Linguistics, 9, 295, 10.1080/07268608908599424

Slobin, 1987, Thinking for speaking, Proceedings of the Berkeley Linguistic Society, 13, 435, 10.3765/bls.v13i0.1826

Tardif, 1996, Nouns are not always learned before verbs, but why? Evidence from Mandarin Chinese, Developmental Psychology, 32, 492, 10.1037/0012-1649.32.3.492

Traugott, 1978, On the expression of spatiotemporal relations in language, 369

Waxman, 1990, Nouns mark category relations: Toddlers' and Preschoolers' word-learning biases, Child Development, 61, 1461, 10.2307/1130756

Whorf, 1956