Core knowledge
Tóm tắt
Từ khóa
Tài liệu tham khảo
Agrillo, 2004, 27th Annual Meeting of the European Conference on Visual Perception (ECVP)
Aguiar, 1999, 2.5-month-old infants’ reasoning about when objects should and should not be occluded, Cognitive Psychology, 39, 116, 10.1006/cogp.1999.0717
Bar-Haim, 2006, Nature and nurture in own-race face processing, Psychological Science, 17, 159, 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01679.x
Baron, 2006, The development of implicit attitudes: evidence of race evaluations from ages 6 and 10 and adulthood, Psychological Science, 17, 53, 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01664.x
Barth, 2003, The construction of large number representations in adults, Cognition, 86, 201, 10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00178-6
Barth, 2005, Abstract number and arithmetic in young children, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 39, 14117
Barth, 2006, Non-symbolic arithmetic in adults and young children, Cognition, 98, 199, 10.1016/j.cognition.2004.09.011
Brannon, 2004, Number bias for the discrimination of large visual sets in infancy, Cognition, 93, B59, 10.1016/j.cognition.2004.01.004
Braudel, 1988, The identity of France
Carey, 1985, Conceptual change in childhood
Carey, 2001, Evolutionary and ontogenetic foundations of arithmetic, Mind and Language, 16, 37, 10.1111/1468-0017.00155
Cheng, 1986, A purely geometric module in the rat's spatial representation, Cognition, 23, 149, 10.1016/0010-0277(86)90041-7
Cheng, 2005, Is there a geometric module for spatial orientation? Squaring theory and evidence, Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 12, 1, 10.3758/BF03196346
Church, 1990, Alternative representations of time, number, and rate, Cognition, 37, 55, 10.1016/0010-0277(90)90018-F
Cosmides, 1994, Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture, 85, 10.1017/CBO9780511752902.005
Cosmides, 2003, Perceptions of race, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 173, 10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00057-3
Cushman, The role of conscious reasoning and intuition in moral judgments: testing three principles of harm, Psychological Science
Dehaene, 1997, The number sense: How the mind creates mathematics
Dehaene, 1993, Development of elementary numerical abilities: a neuronal model, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 390, 10.1162/jocn.1993.5.4.390
Dehaene, 2006, Core knowledge of geometry in an Amazonian indigene group, Science, 311, 381, 10.1126/science.1121739
Everett, 2005, Cultural constraints on grammar and cognition in Pirahã: another look at the design features of human language, Current Anthropology, 46, 621, 10.1086/431525
Feigenson, 2003, Tracking individuals via object-files: evidence from infants’ manual search, Developmental Science, 6, 568, 10.1111/1467-7687.00313
Feigenson, 2004, Core systems of number, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 307, 10.1016/j.tics.2004.05.002
Flombaum, 2005, Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) spontaneously compute addition operations over large numbers, Cognition, 97, 315, 10.1016/j.cognition.2004.09.004
Flombaum, 2005, Rhesus monkeys attribute perceptions to others, Current Biology, 15, 447, 10.1016/j.cub.2004.12.076
Fodor, 2000, The mind doesn't work that way: The scope and limits of computational psychology, 10.7551/mitpress/4627.001.0001
Gallistel, 1992, Preverbal and verbal counting and computation, Cognition, 44, 43, 10.1016/0010-0277(92)90050-R
Gelman, 1991, The epigenesis of mind: Essays on biology and cognition, 293
Gergely, 2003, Teleological reasoning in infancy: the naïve theory of rational action, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 287, 10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00128-1
Gordon, 2004, Numerical cognition without words: evidence from Amazonia, Science, 306, 496, 10.1126/science.1094492
Gouteux, 2001, Children's use of geometry and landmarks to reorient in an open space, Cognition, 81, 119, 10.1016/S0010-0277(01)00128-7
Hare, 2001, Do chimpanzees know what conspecifics know?, Animal Behavior, 61, 139, 10.1006/anbe.2000.1518
Hauser, 2003, Spontaneous representations of small numbers of objects by rhesus macaques: examinations of content and format, Cognitive Psychology, 47, 367, 10.1016/S0010-0285(03)00050-1
Hauser, 2003, Evolutionary foundations of number: spontaneous representation of numerical magnitudes by cotton-top tamarins, Proceedings of the Royal Society, London, B, 270, 1441, 10.1098/rspb.2003.2414
Hawking, 2002, On the shoulders of giants: The great works of physics and astronomy
Hermer, 1996, Modularity and development: the case of spatial reorientation, Cognition, 61, 195, 10.1016/S0010-0277(96)00714-7
Hermer-Vazquez, 1999, Sources of flexibility in human cognition: dual-task studies of space and language, Cognitive Psychology, 39, 3, 10.1006/cogp.1998.0713
Hinton, 1993, Mind and brain: Readings from the Scientific American magazine, 113
Hood, 1998, Adults’ eyes trigger shifts of visual attention in human infants, Psychological Science, 9, 131, 10.1111/1467-9280.00024
Huntley-Fenner, 2002, Objects are individuals but stuff doesn't count: perceived rigidity and cohesiveness influence infants’ representations of small groups of discrete entities, Cognition, 85, 203, 10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00088-4
Iacoboni, 1999, Cortical mechanisms of human imitation, Science, 286, 2526, 10.1126/science.286.5449.2526
Izard, V. (2006). Interactions entre les representations numeriques verbales et non-verbales: études théoriques et expérimentales. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Université Paris VI, Paris.
Izard, V. , & Dehaene, S. (2006) Calibrating the number line.
Johnson, 2001, Inferring the goals of a nonhuman agent, Cognitive Development, 16, 637, 10.1016/S0885-2014(01)00043-0
Johnson, 1998, Whose gaze will infants follow? The elicitation of gaze-following in 12-month-olds, Developmental Science, 1, 233, 10.1111/1467-7687.00036
Kelly, 2005, Three-month-olds, but not newborns, prefer own-race faces, Developmental Science, 8, F31, 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.0434a.x
Keil, 1998, Two dogmas of conceptual empiricism: implications for hybrid models of the structure of knowledge, Cognition, 65, 103, 10.1016/S0010-0277(97)00041-3
Kinzler, K.D. , & Spelke, E.S. (2005). The effect of language on infants’ preference for faces. Poster presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Atlanta, GA.
Kuhlmeier, 2004, Do 5-month-old infants see humans as material objects?, Cognition, 94, 95, 10.1016/j.cognition.2004.02.007
Lavin, 2001, Domain effects in lexical development: learning words for foods and toys, Cognitive Development, 16, 929, 10.1016/S0885-2014(02)00070-9
Lea, 1996, Perception of object unity in chicks: a comparison with the human infant, Infant Behavior and Development, 19, 501, 10.1016/S0163-6383(96)90010-7
Lee, Reorientation and landmark-guided search by young children: evidence for two systems, Psychological Science
Leslie, 1987, Do six-month-old infants perceive causality?, Cognition, 25, 265, 10.1016/S0010-0277(87)80006-9
Leslie, 1998, Indexing and the object concept: developing ‘what’ and ‘where’ systems, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 10, 10.1016/S1364-6613(97)01113-3
Lipton, 2003, Origins of number sense: large-number discrimination in human infants, Psychological Science, 14, 396, 10.1111/1467-9280.01453
Lipton, 2004, Discrimination of large and small numerosities by human infants, Infancy, 5, 271, 10.1207/s15327078in0503_2
Locke, 1689, An essay concerning human understanding
Lourenco, 2005, Toddlers’ representations of space, Psychological Science, 16, 255, 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01524.x
McCloskey, 1983, Intuitive physics, Scientific American, 248, 122, 10.1038/scientificamerican0483-122
McCrink, 2004, Large-number addition and subtraction by 9-month-old infants, Psychological Science, 15, 776, 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00755.x
McKee, C. (2006). The effect of social information on infants’ food preferences. Unpublished honors thesis, Harvard University, April 2006 ( K. Shutts , K. Kinzler , and E. Spelke , advisors).
Margules, 1988, Heading in the rat: determination by environmental shape, Animal Learning and Behavior, 16, 404, 10.3758/BF03209379
Marino, 2005, The role of closure in defining the ‘objects’ of object-based attention, Perception and Psychophysics, 67, 1140, 10.3758/BF03193547
Meck, 1983, A mode control model of counting and timing processes, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 9, 320
Mehler, 1988, A precursor of language acquisition in young infants, Cognition, 29, 143, 10.1016/0010-0277(88)90035-2
Meltzoff, 1995, Understanding the intentions of others: re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children, Developmental Psychology, 31, 838, 10.1037/0012-1649.31.5.838
Meltzoff, 1977, Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates, Science, 198, 75, 10.1126/science.198.4312.75
Moon, 1993, Two-day-olds prefer their native language, Infant Behavior and Development, 16, 495, 10.1016/0163-6383(93)80007-U
Newcombe, 2005, Action as an organizer of learning and development, 221
Newcombe, 2000, Making space: The development of spatial representation and reasoning, 10.7551/mitpress/4395.001.0001
Pettigrew, T.F. , & Tropp, L.R. (2006). A meta-analytic test of intergoup contact theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.
Pica, 2004, Exact and approximate arithmetic in an Amazonian indigene group, Science, 306, 499, 10.1126/science.1102085
Pinker, 2002, The blank slate: The modern denial of human nature
Quinn, 2002, Representation of the gender of human faces by infants: a preference for female, Perception, 31, 1109, 10.1068/p3331
Randall, 2005, Warped passages
Regolin, 1995, Perception of partly occluded objects by young chicks, Perception and Psychophysics, 57, 971, 10.3758/BF03205456
Rizzolatti, 2002, Motor and cognitive functions of the ventral premotor cortex, Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 12, 149, 10.1016/S0959-4388(02)00308-2
Rosenberg, R. , & Carey, S. (2006). Infants’ indexing of objects vs. non-cohesive entities. Poster presented at the Biennial meeting of the International Society for Infant Studies.
Rumelhart, 1985, Distributed memory and the representation of general and specific information, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114, 159, 10.1037/0096-3445.114.2.159
Santos, 2004, ‘Core knowledges’: a dissociation between spatiotemporal knowledge and contact-mechanics in a non-human primate?, Developmental Science, 7, 167, 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00335.x
Santos, 2001, 82, 127
Scholl, 2001, Objects and attention: the state of the art, Cognition, 80, 1, 10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00152-9
Scholl, 1999, Tracking multiple items through occlusion: clues to visual objecthood, Cognitive Psychology, 2, 259, 10.1006/cogp.1998.0698
Scholl, 2001, What is a visual object? Evidence from target merging in multiple object tracking, Cognition, 80, 159, 10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00157-8
Shutts, K. (2006). Properties of infants’ learning about objects. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Harvard University.
Spelke, 2003, Language in mind
Spelke, 2004, Attention and performance, vol. 20: Functional neuroimaging of visual cognition
Spelke, 1995, Causal cognition: A multidisciplinary debate, 44
Spelke, 1990, Principles of object perception, Cognitive Science, 14, 29, 10.1207/s15516709cog1401_3
Tomasello, 1999, The cultural origins of human cognition
Valenza, Perceptual completion in newborn human infants, Child Development
Van Oeffelen, 1982, A probabilistic model for the discrimination of visual number, Perception and Psychophysics, 32, 163, 10.3758/BF03204275
vanMarle, 2003, Attentive tracking of objects versus substances, Psychological Science, 14, 498, 10.1111/1467-9280.03451
Vishton, P.M. , Stulac, S.N. , & Calhoun, E.K. (1998). Using young infants’ tendency to reach for object boundaries to explore perception of connectedness: rectangles, ovals, and faces. Paper presented at the International Conference on Infant Studies, Atlanta, Georgia.
Wang, 1999, Mechanisms of reorientation and object localization by children: a comparison with rats, Behavioral Neuroscience, 113, 475, 10.1037/0735-7044.113.3.475
Watson, 1972, Smiling, cooing, and ‘The Game’, Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 18, 323
Wood, 2005, Infants’ enumeration of actions: numerical discrimination and its signature limits, Developmental Science, 8, 173, 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00404.x
Woodward, 1998, Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach, Cognition, 69, 1, 10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00058-4
Woodward, 1999, Infants’ ability to distinguish between purposeful and non-purposeful behaviors, Infant Behavior and Development, 22, 145, 10.1016/S0163-6383(99)00007-7
Xu, 2000, Large number discrimination in 6-month-old infants, Cognition, 74, B1, 10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00066-9