Core knowledge

Developmental Science - Tập 10 Số 1 - Trang 89-96 - 2007
Elizabeth S. Spelke1, Katherine D. Kinzler1
1Department of Psychology, Harvard University, USA

Tóm tắt

Từ khóa


Tài liệu tham khảo

Agrillo, 2004, 27th Annual Meeting of the European Conference on Visual Perception (ECVP)

Aguiar, 1999, 2.5-month-old infants’ reasoning about when objects should and should not be occluded, Cognitive Psychology, 39, 116, 10.1006/cogp.1999.0717

Bar-Haim, 2006, Nature and nurture in own-race face processing, Psychological Science, 17, 159, 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01679.x

Baron, 2006, The development of implicit attitudes: evidence of race evaluations from ages 6 and 10 and adulthood, Psychological Science, 17, 53, 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01664.x

Barth, 2003, The construction of large number representations in adults, Cognition, 86, 201, 10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00178-6

Barth, 2005, Abstract number and arithmetic in young children, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 39, 14117

Barth, 2006, Non-symbolic arithmetic in adults and young children, Cognition, 98, 199, 10.1016/j.cognition.2004.09.011

Bloom, 2000, How children learn the meanings of words, 10.7551/mitpress/3577.001.0001

Brannon, 2004, Number bias for the discrimination of large visual sets in infancy, Cognition, 93, B59, 10.1016/j.cognition.2004.01.004

Braudel, 1988, The identity of France

Carey, 1985, Conceptual change in childhood

Carey, 2001, Evolutionary and ontogenetic foundations of arithmetic, Mind and Language, 16, 37, 10.1111/1468-0017.00155

Cheng, 1986, A purely geometric module in the rat's spatial representation, Cognition, 23, 149, 10.1016/0010-0277(86)90041-7

Cheng, 2005, Is there a geometric module for spatial orientation? Squaring theory and evidence, Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 12, 1, 10.3758/BF03196346

Church, 1990, Alternative representations of time, number, and rate, Cognition, 37, 55, 10.1016/0010-0277(90)90018-F

Cosmides, 1994, Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture, 85, 10.1017/CBO9780511752902.005

Cosmides, 2003, Perceptions of race, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 173, 10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00057-3

Cushman, The role of conscious reasoning and intuition in moral judgments: testing three principles of harm, Psychological Science

Darwin, 1871, The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex, 10.1037/12293-000

Dehaene, 1997, The number sense: How the mind creates mathematics

Dehaene, 1993, Development of elementary numerical abilities: a neuronal model, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 390, 10.1162/jocn.1993.5.4.390

Dehaene, 2006, Core knowledge of geometry in an Amazonian indigene group, Science, 311, 381, 10.1126/science.1121739

Everett, 2005, Cultural constraints on grammar and cognition in Pirahã: another look at the design features of human language, Current Anthropology, 46, 621, 10.1086/431525

Farroni, 2004, Gaze following in newborns, Infancy, 5, 39, 10.1207/s15327078in0501_2

Feigenson, 2003, Tracking individuals via object-files: evidence from infants’ manual search, Developmental Science, 6, 568, 10.1111/1467-7687.00313

Feigenson, 2004, Core systems of number, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 307, 10.1016/j.tics.2004.05.002

Flombaum, 2005, Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) spontaneously compute addition operations over large numbers, Cognition, 97, 315, 10.1016/j.cognition.2004.09.004

Flombaum, 2005, Rhesus monkeys attribute perceptions to others, Current Biology, 15, 447, 10.1016/j.cub.2004.12.076

Fodor, 2000, The mind doesn't work that way: The scope and limits of computational psychology, 10.7551/mitpress/4627.001.0001

Gallistel, 1992, Preverbal and verbal counting and computation, Cognition, 44, 43, 10.1016/0010-0277(92)90050-R

Gelman, 1991, The epigenesis of mind: Essays on biology and cognition, 293

Gergely, 2003, Teleological reasoning in infancy: the naïve theory of rational action, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 287, 10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00128-1

Gordon, 2004, Numerical cognition without words: evidence from Amazonia, Science, 306, 496, 10.1126/science.1094492

Gouteux, 2001, Children's use of geometry and landmarks to reorient in an open space, Cognition, 81, 119, 10.1016/S0010-0277(01)00128-7

Hare, 2001, Do chimpanzees know what conspecifics know?, Animal Behavior, 61, 139, 10.1006/anbe.2000.1518

Hauser, 2003, Spontaneous representations of small numbers of objects by rhesus macaques: examinations of content and format, Cognitive Psychology, 47, 367, 10.1016/S0010-0285(03)00050-1

Hauser, 2003, Evolutionary foundations of number: spontaneous representation of numerical magnitudes by cotton-top tamarins, Proceedings of the Royal Society, London, B, 270, 1441, 10.1098/rspb.2003.2414

Hawking, 2002, On the shoulders of giants: The great works of physics and astronomy

Hermer, 1996, Modularity and development: the case of spatial reorientation, Cognition, 61, 195, 10.1016/S0010-0277(96)00714-7

Hermer-Vazquez, 1999, Sources of flexibility in human cognition: dual-task studies of space and language, Cognitive Psychology, 39, 3, 10.1006/cogp.1998.0713

Hinton, 1993, Mind and brain: Readings from the Scientific American magazine, 113

Hood, 1998, Adults’ eyes trigger shifts of visual attention in human infants, Psychological Science, 9, 131, 10.1111/1467-9280.00024

Hume, 1748, An enquiry concerning human understanding, 10.1093/oseo/instance.00032980

Huntley-Fenner, 2002, Objects are individuals but stuff doesn't count: perceived rigidity and cohesiveness influence infants’ representations of small groups of discrete entities, Cognition, 85, 203, 10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00088-4

Iacoboni, 1999, Cortical mechanisms of human imitation, Science, 286, 2526, 10.1126/science.286.5449.2526

Izard, V. (2006). Interactions entre les representations numeriques verbales et non-verbales: études théoriques et expérimentales. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Université Paris VI, Paris.

Izard, V. , & Dehaene, S. (2006) Calibrating the number line.

Johnson, 2001, Inferring the goals of a nonhuman agent, Cognitive Development, 16, 637, 10.1016/S0885-2014(01)00043-0

Johnson, 1998, Whose gaze will infants follow? The elicitation of gaze-following in 12-month-olds, Developmental Science, 1, 233, 10.1111/1467-7687.00036

Kelly, 2005, Three-month-olds, but not newborns, prefer own-race faces, Developmental Science, 8, F31, 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.0434a.x

Keil, 1998, Two dogmas of conceptual empiricism: implications for hybrid models of the structure of knowledge, Cognition, 65, 103, 10.1016/S0010-0277(97)00041-3

Kinzler, K.D. , & Spelke, E.S. (2005). The effect of language on infants’ preference for faces. Poster presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Atlanta, GA.

Kuhlmeier, 2004, Do 5-month-old infants see humans as material objects?, Cognition, 94, 95, 10.1016/j.cognition.2004.02.007

Lavin, 2001, Domain effects in lexical development: learning words for foods and toys, Cognitive Development, 16, 929, 10.1016/S0885-2014(02)00070-9

Lea, 1996, Perception of object unity in chicks: a comparison with the human infant, Infant Behavior and Development, 19, 501, 10.1016/S0163-6383(96)90010-7

Lee, Reorientation and landmark-guided search by young children: evidence for two systems, Psychological Science

Leslie, 1987, Do six-month-old infants perceive causality?, Cognition, 25, 265, 10.1016/S0010-0277(87)80006-9

Leslie, 1998, Indexing and the object concept: developing ‘what’ and ‘where’ systems, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 10, 10.1016/S1364-6613(97)01113-3

Lipton, 2003, Origins of number sense: large-number discrimination in human infants, Psychological Science, 14, 396, 10.1111/1467-9280.01453

Lipton, 2004, Discrimination of large and small numerosities by human infants, Infancy, 5, 271, 10.1207/s15327078in0503_2

Locke, 1689, An essay concerning human understanding

Lourenco, 2005, Toddlers’ representations of space, Psychological Science, 16, 255, 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01524.x

McCloskey, 1983, Intuitive physics, Scientific American, 248, 122, 10.1038/scientificamerican0483-122

McCrink, 2004, Large-number addition and subtraction by 9-month-old infants, Psychological Science, 15, 776, 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00755.x

McKee, C. (2006). The effect of social information on infants’ food preferences. Unpublished honors thesis, Harvard University, April 2006 ( K. Shutts , K. Kinzler , and E. Spelke , advisors).

Margules, 1988, Heading in the rat: determination by environmental shape, Animal Learning and Behavior, 16, 404, 10.3758/BF03209379

Marino, 2005, The role of closure in defining the ‘objects’ of object-based attention, Perception and Psychophysics, 67, 1140, 10.3758/BF03193547

Meck, 1983, A mode control model of counting and timing processes, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 9, 320

Mehler, 1988, A precursor of language acquisition in young infants, Cognition, 29, 143, 10.1016/0010-0277(88)90035-2

Meltzoff, 1995, Understanding the intentions of others: re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children, Developmental Psychology, 31, 838, 10.1037/0012-1649.31.5.838

Meltzoff, 1977, Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates, Science, 198, 75, 10.1126/science.198.4312.75

Moon, 1993, Two-day-olds prefer their native language, Infant Behavior and Development, 16, 495, 10.1016/0163-6383(93)80007-U

Newcombe, 2005, Action as an organizer of learning and development, 221

Newcombe, 2000, Making space: The development of spatial representation and reasoning, 10.7551/mitpress/4395.001.0001

Pettigrew, T.F. , & Tropp, L.R. (2006). A meta-analytic test of intergoup contact theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.

Pica, 2004, Exact and approximate arithmetic in an Amazonian indigene group, Science, 306, 499, 10.1126/science.1102085

Pinker, 2002, The blank slate: The modern denial of human nature

Quinn, 2002, Representation of the gender of human faces by infants: a preference for female, Perception, 31, 1109, 10.1068/p3331

Randall, 2005, Warped passages

Regolin, 1995, Perception of partly occluded objects by young chicks, Perception and Psychophysics, 57, 971, 10.3758/BF03205456

Rizzolatti, 2002, Motor and cognitive functions of the ventral premotor cortex, Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 12, 149, 10.1016/S0959-4388(02)00308-2

Rosenberg, R. , & Carey, S. (2006). Infants’ indexing of objects vs. non-cohesive entities. Poster presented at the Biennial meeting of the International Society for Infant Studies.

Rumelhart, 1985, Distributed memory and the representation of general and specific information, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114, 159, 10.1037/0096-3445.114.2.159

Santos, 2004, ‘Core knowledges’: a dissociation between spatiotemporal knowledge and contact-mechanics in a non-human primate?, Developmental Science, 7, 167, 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00335.x

Santos, 2001, 82, 127

Scholl, 2001, Objects and attention: the state of the art, Cognition, 80, 1, 10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00152-9

Scholl, 1999, Tracking multiple items through occlusion: clues to visual objecthood, Cognitive Psychology, 2, 259, 10.1006/cogp.1998.0698

Scholl, 2001, What is a visual object? Evidence from target merging in multiple object tracking, Cognition, 80, 159, 10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00157-8

Shell, 2001, Language wars, CR: The New Centennial Review, 1, 1, 10.1353/ncr.2003.0059

Shutts, K. (2006). Properties of infants’ learning about objects. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Harvard University.

Spelke, 2000, Core knowledge, American Psychologist, 55, 1233, 10.1037/0003-066X.55.11.1233

Spelke, 2003, Language in mind

Spelke, 2004, Attention and performance, vol. 20: Functional neuroimaging of visual cognition

Spelke, 1995, Causal cognition: A multidisciplinary debate, 44

Spelke, 1990, Principles of object perception, Cognitive Science, 14, 29, 10.1207/s15516709cog1401_3

Tomasello, 1999, The cultural origins of human cognition

Valenza, Perceptual completion in newborn human infants, Child Development

Van Oeffelen, 1982, A probabilistic model for the discrimination of visual number, Perception and Psychophysics, 32, 163, 10.3758/BF03204275

vanMarle, 2003, Attentive tracking of objects versus substances, Psychological Science, 14, 498, 10.1111/1467-9280.03451

Vishton, P.M. , Stulac, S.N. , & Calhoun, E.K. (1998). Using young infants’ tendency to reach for object boundaries to explore perception of connectedness: rectangles, ovals, and faces. Paper presented at the International Conference on Infant Studies, Atlanta, Georgia.

Wang, 1999, Mechanisms of reorientation and object localization by children: a comparison with rats, Behavioral Neuroscience, 113, 475, 10.1037/0735-7044.113.3.475

Watson, 1972, Smiling, cooing, and ‘The Game’, Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 18, 323

Wood, 2005, Infants’ enumeration of actions: numerical discrimination and its signature limits, Developmental Science, 8, 173, 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00404.x

Woodward, 1998, Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach, Cognition, 69, 1, 10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00058-4

Woodward, 1999, Infants’ ability to distinguish between purposeful and non-purposeful behaviors, Infant Behavior and Development, 22, 145, 10.1016/S0163-6383(99)00007-7

Wynn, 1990, Children's understanding of counting, Cognition, 36, 155, 10.1016/0010-0277(90)90003-3

Xu, 2000, Large number discrimination in 6-month-old infants, Cognition, 74, B1, 10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00066-9

Xu, 2005, Number sense in human infants, Developmental Science, 8, 88, 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00395.x