Context and Behavioral Processes in Extinction: Table 1.
Tóm tắt
This article provides a selective review and integration of the behavioral literature on Pavlovian extinction. The first part reviews evidence that extinction does not destroy the original learning, but instead generates new learning that is especially context-dependent. The second part examines insights provided by research on several related behavioral phenomena (the interference paradigms, conditioned inhibition, and inhibition despite reinforcement). The final part examines four potential causes of extinction: the discrimination of a new reinforcement rate, generalization decrement, response inhibition, and violation of a reinforcer expectation. The data are consistent with behavioral models that emphasize the role of generalization decrement and expectation violation, but would be more so if those models were expanded to better accommodate the finding that extinction involves a context-modulated form of inhibitory learning.
Từ khóa
Tài liệu tham khảo
1991, Q. J. Exp. Psychol. B, 43, 199
____. 1991. Context and retrieval in extinction and in other examples of interference in simple associative learning. In: Current topics in animal learning: Brain, emotion, and cognition (eds. L. Dachowski and C.F. Flaherty), pp. 25 -53. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.
Bouton, M.E. and Nelson, J.B. 1998. Mechanisms of feature-positive and feature-negative discrimination learning in an appetitive conditioning paradigm. In Occasion setting: Associative learning and cognition in animals (eds. N. Schmajuk and P.C. Holland), pp. 69 -112. American Psychological Association, Washington, DC.
1993, Q. J. Exp. Psychol., 46B, 63
Bouton, M.E., Woods, A.M., and Pineño, O. 2004. Occasional reinforced trials during extinction can slow the rate of rapid reacquisition. Learn. Motivat. (in press).
Brandon, S.E. and Wagner, A.R. 1998. Occasion setting: Influences of conditioned emotional responses and configural cues. In Occasion setting: Associative learning and cognition in animals (eds. N.A. Schmajuk and P.C. Holland), pp. 343 -382. American Psychological Association, Washington, DC.
2000, Q. J. Exp. Psychol., 53B, 25
2004, Q. J. Exp. Psychol., 57B, 97
1993, Hippocampus, 3, 492
Hall, G. and Mondragón, E. 1998. Contextual control as occasion setting. In Occasion setting: Associative learning and cognition in animals (eds. N. Schmajuk and P.C. Holland), pp. 199-222). American Psychological Association, Washington, DC.
1975, Handbook of learning and cognitive processes: Conditioning and behavior theory, 2, 99
Konorski, J. 1948. Conditioned reflexes and neuron organization. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK.
____. 1967. Integrative activity of the brain. University of Chicago Press, Chicago.
Mackintosh, N.J. 1974. The psychology of animal learning. Academic Press, New York.
Overton, D.A. 1985. Contextual stimulus effects of drugs and internal states. In Context and learning (eds. P.D. Balsam and A. Tomie), pp. 357-384. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.
Pavlov, I.P. 1927. Conditioned reflexes. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK.
1980, Psychol. Rev., 87, 332
1996, Q. J. Exp. Psychol., 49B, 245
1997, Q. J. Exp. Psychol., 50B, 238
____. 2001. Experimental extinction. In Handbook of contemporary learning theories (eds. R.R. Mowrer and S.B. Klein), pp. 119-154. Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ.
Rescorla, R.A. and Wagner, A.R. 1972. A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory (eds. A.H. Black and W.K. Prokasy), pp. 64 -99. Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York.
Schmajuk, N.A. and Holland, P.C., eds. 1998. Occasion-setting: Associative learning and cognition in animals. American Psychological Association, Washington, DC.
Spear, N.E. 1981. Extending the domain of memory retrieval. In Information processing in animals: Memory mechanisms (eds. N.E. Spear and R.R. Miller), pp. 341 -378. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.
Staddon, J E.R. 1988. Learning as inference. In Evolution and learning (eds. R.C. Bolles and M.D. Beecher), pp. 59-78. Erbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.
2000, Int. J. Comp. Psychol., 13, 137
1969, The psychology of learning and motivation, 3, 1
____. 1981. SOP: A model of automatic memory processing in animal behavior. In Information processing in animals: Memory mechanisms (eds. N.E. Spears and R.R. Miller) pp. 5 -47. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.
Wagner, A.R. and Brandon, S.E. 1989. Evolution of a structured connectionist model of Pavlovian conditioning (AESOP). In Contemporary learning theories: Pavlovian conditioning and the status of traditional learning theory (eds. S.B. Klein and R.R. Mowrer), pp. 149-189. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ.
____. 2001. A componential theory of Pavlovian conditioning. In Handbook of contemporary learning theories (eds. R.R. Mowrer and S.B. Klein), pp. 23 -64. Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ.