Comparison of Bayesian and regression approaches to the study of information processing in judgment
Tài liệu tham khảo
Agnew, 1969
Anderson, 1959, Test of a model for opinion change, Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 59, 371, 10.1037/h0042539
Anderson, 1962, Application of an additive model to impression formation, Science, 138, 817, 10.1126/science.138.3542.817
Anderson, 1964, Test of a model for number-averaging behavior, Psychonomic Science, 1, 191, 10.3758/BF03342858
Anderson, 1965, Averaging versus adding as a stimulus-combination rule in impression formation, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70, 394, 10.1037/h0022280
Anderson, 1965, Primacy effects in personality impression formation using a generalized order effect paradigm, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2, 1, 10.1037/h0021966
Anderson, 1966, Component ratings in impression formation, Psychonomic Science, 6, 279, 10.3758/BF03328066
Anderson, 1967, Application of a weighted average model to a psychophysical averaging task, Psychonomic Science, 8, 227, 10.3758/BF03331634
Anderson, 1967, Averaging model analysis of set-size effect in impression formation, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 75, 158, 10.1037/h0024995
Anderson, 1968, Application of a linear-serial model to a personality-impression task using serial presentation, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 10, 354, 10.1037/h0026816
Anderson, 1968, A simple model for information integration
Anderson, 1969, Comment on “An analysis-of-variance model for the assessment of configural cue utilization in clinical judgment”, Psychological Bulletin, 72, 63, 10.1037/h0027420
Anderson, 1970, Functional measurement and psychophysical judgment, Psychological Review, 77, 153, 10.1037/h0029064
Anderson, N. H. Integration theory and attitude change. Psychological Review, in press.
Anderson, 1971, Two more tests against change of meaning in adjective combinations, Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 10, 75, 10.1016/S0022-5371(71)80097-X
Anderson, 1961, Primacy effects in personality impression formation, Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 63, 346, 10.1037/h0046719
Anderson, 1963, Effects of concomitant verbal recall on order effects in personality impression formation, Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 2, 379, 10.1016/S0022-5371(63)80039-0
Anderson, 1965, Effect of stimulus inconsistency and discounting instructions in personality impression formation, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2, 531, 10.1037/h0022484
Anderson, 1968, Further data on a weighted average model for judgment in a lifted weight task, Perception and Psychophysics, 4, 81, 10.3758/BF03209513
Anderson, 1965, Effect of context on ratings of personality traits, Psychonomic Science, 3, 433, 10.3758/BF03343217
Anderson, 1964, Order effects in impression formation in four classes of stimuli, Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 69, 467, 10.1037/h0047472
Anderson, 1970, Information integration in risky decision making, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 84, 441, 10.1037/h0029300
Armelius, 1970, Effect of cue intercorrelation in a multiple cue probability learning task with different cue validities
Asch, 1946, Forming impressions of personality, Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 41, 258, 10.1037/h0055756
Azuma, 1966, Cue-response correlations in the attainment of a scalar concept, The American Journal of Psychology, 79, 38, 10.2307/1420706
Beach, 1966, Accuracy and consistency in the revision of subjective probabilities, IEEE Transactions on Human Factors in Electronics, 7, 29, 10.1109/THFE.1966.231981
Beach, 1968, Probability magnitudes and conservative revision of subjective probabilities, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 77, 57, 10.1037/h0025800
Beach, 1967, Data sequences and subjective sampling distributions, Psychonomic Science, 9, 309, 10.3758/BF03327820
Beach, 1967, Subjective probabilities inferred from estimates and bets, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 75, 354, 10.1037/h0025061
Beach, 1969, Subjective probability and decision strategy, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 79, 133, 10.1037/h0026959
Beach, 1969, Subjective probability revision and subsequent decisions, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 561, 10.1037/h0027919
Beach, 1970, Sample proportion and subjective probability revisions, Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 5, 183, 10.1016/0030-5073(70)90014-0
Bieri, 1966
Björkman, 1965, Learning of linear functions: Comparison between a positive and a negative slope, Report No. 183 from the Psychological Laboratories of the University of Stockholm
Björkman, 1968, The effect of training and number of stimuli on the response variance in correlation learning
Booartz, 1970, Transforming response measures to remove interactions or other sources of variance, Psychonomic Science, 19, 87, 10.3758/BF03337434
Bowman, 1963, Consistency and optimality in managerial decision making, Management Science, 9, 310, 10.1287/mnsc.9.2.310
Brehmer, 1969, Cognitive dependence on additive and configural cue-criterion relations, The American Journal of Psychology, 82, 490, 10.2307/1420442
Brehmer, 1969, The roles of policy differences and inconsistency in policy conflict
Brehmer, 1969
Brehmer, 1970, Inference behavior in a situation where the cues are not reliably perceived, Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 5, 330, 10.1016/0030-5073(70)90024-3
Brehmer, 1970, The relation between cue dependency and cue validity in single-cue probability learning with scaled cue and criterion variables, Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 5, 542, 10.1016/0030-5073(70)90039-5
Briggs, 1965, Automated Bayesian hypothesis-selection in a simulated threat-diagnosis system, 169
Brody, 1965, The effect of commitment to correct and incorrect decisions on confidence in a sequential decision-task, American Journal of Psychology, 78, 251, 10.2307/1420497
Brown, 1970, The judgment of suicide lethality: A comparison of judgmental models obtained under contrived versus natural conditions
Bruner, 1956
Brunswik, 1952
Brunswik, 1955, Representative design and probabilistic theory in a functional psychology, Psychological Review, 62, 193, 10.1037/h0047470
Brunswik, 1956
Carroll, 1963, Functional learning: The learning of continuous functional mappings relating stimulus and response continua, 10.1002/j.2333-8504.1963.tb00958.x
Christal, 1963, JAN: A technique for analyzing group judgment, 10.21236/AD0403813
Clarkson, 1962
Cochran, 1957
Cohen, 1968, Multiple regression as a general data-analytic system, Psychological Bulletin, 70, 426, 10.1037/h0026714
Coombs, 1964
Coombs, 1970, Polynomial psychophysics of risk, Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 7, 317, 10.1016/0022-2496(70)90051-9
Dale, 1968, Weighing evidence: An attempt to assess the efficiency of the human operator, Ergonomics, 11, 215, 10.1080/00140136808930967
Darlington, 1968, Multiple regression in psychological research and practice, Psychological Bulletin, 69, 161, 10.1037/h0025471
Dawes, 1964, Social selection based on multidimensional criteria, Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 68, 104, 10.1037/h0047832
Dawes, 1971, A case study of graduate admissions: Application of three principles of human decision making, American Psychologist, 26, 180, 10.1037/h0030868
Dawes, 1970, The prediction of bootstrapping and a method of amalgamation, Oregon Research Institute Research Bulletin, Vol. 10
Ducharme, 1970, A response bias explanation of conservative human inference, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 85, 66, 10.1037/h0029546
Ducharme, 1968, Intuitive inference about normally distributed populations, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 78, 269, 10.1037/h0026309
Dudycha, 1970, A Monte Carlo evaluation of JAN: A technique for capturing and clustering rater's policies, Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 5, 501, 10.1016/0030-5073(70)90037-1
Dudycha, 1966, The effect of variations in the cue R matrix upon the obtained policy equation of judges, Educational and Psychological Measurement, 26, 583, 10.1177/001316446602600304
Dudycha, 1966, Characteristics of the human inference process in complex choice behavior situations, Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 1, 110, 10.1016/0030-5073(66)90008-0
Dustin, 1966, Redundancy in impression formation, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 3, 500, 10.1037/h0023293
Earle, 1970, Task learning, interpersonal learning, and cognitive complexity, Oregon Research Institute Research Bulletin, Vol. 10
Eckenrode, 1965, Weighting multiple criteria, Management Science, 12, 180, 10.1287/mnsc.12.3.180
Edwards, 1954, The theory of decision making, Psychological Bulletin, 51, 380, 10.1037/h0053870
Edwards, 1962, Dynamic decision theory and probabilistic information processing, Human Factors, 4, 59, 10.1177/001872086200400201
Edwards, 1965, Optimal strategies for seeking information: Models for statistics, choice reaction times, and human information processing, Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 2, 312, 10.1016/0022-2496(65)90007-6
Edwards, 1966, Nonconservative probabilistic information processing systems, Report from Decision Sciences Laboratory, Electronic Systems Division, Air Force Systems Command, USAF, ESD-TR-66-404
Edwards, 1968, Conservatism in human information processing
Edwards, 1965, Emerging technologies for making decisions
Edwards, 1963, Bayesian statistical inference for psychological research, Psychological Review, 70, 193, 10.1037/h0044139
Edwards, 1964, Man as transducer for probabilities in Bayesian command and control systems
Edwards, 1968, Probabilistic information processing systems: Design and evaluation, IEEE Transactions on Systems Science and Cybernetics, Vol. SSC-4, 248, 10.1109/TSSC.1968.300119
Einhorn, 1970, The use of nonlinear, noncompensatory models in decision making, Psychological Bulletin, 73, 221, 10.1037/h0028695
Einhorn, 1971, Use of nonlinear, noncompensatory models as a function of task and amount of information, Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 6, 1, 10.1016/0030-5073(71)90002-X
Estes, 1959, The statistical approach to learning theory, II
Fishbein, 1964, Summation versus balance in attitude organization and change, Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 69, 505, 10.1037/h0048533
Fitts, 1954, S-R compatibility: Correspondence among paired elements within stimulus and response codes, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48, 483, 10.1037/h0054967
Fried, 1969, Information seeking: Optional vs. fixed stopping, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 80, 525, 10.1037/h0027484
Galanter, 1967, Some invariances of the iso-sensitivity function and their implications for the utility function of money, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 73, 333, 10.1037/h0024275
Geller, 1968, Confidence and decision speed in the revision of opinion, Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 3, 190, 10.1016/0030-5073(68)90005-6
Gettys, 1968, The probability of an event and estimates of posterior probability based upon its occurrence, Psychonomic Science, 11, 47, 10.3758/BF03330983
Gibson, 1964, The modifiability of decisions made in a changing environment, Report from Decision Sciences Laboratory, Electronic Systems Division, Air Force Systems Command, USAF, ESD-TR-64-657
Goldberg, 1968, Simple models or simple processes? Some research on clinical judgments, American Psychologist, 23, 483, 10.1037/h0026206
Goldberg, 1970, Man versus model of man: A rationale, plus some evidence, for a method of improving on clinical inferences, Psychological Bulletin, 73, 422, 10.1037/h0029230
Gollob, 1968, Impression formation and word combination in sentences, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 10, 341, 10.1037/h0026822
Gordon, 1966, A multi-model analysis of an aggregate scheduling decision
Gray, 1968, Predicting with intuitive correlations, Psychonomic Science, 11, 41, 10.3758/BF03330979
Gray, 1965, The process of prediction as a function of the correlation between two scaled variables, Psychonomic Science, 3, 231, 10.3758/BF03343111
Green, 1968, Descriptions and explanations: A comment on papers by Hoffman and Edwards
Green, 1966
Grinnell, 1970, Bayesian predictions of faculty judgments of graduate school success
Gustafson, 1969, Evaluation of probabilistic information processing in medical decision making, Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 4, 20, 10.1016/0030-5073(69)90029-4
Gustafson, 1969, Subjective probabilities in medical diagnosis, IEEE Transactions on Man-Machine Systems, MMS-10, 61, 10.1109/TMMS.1969.299884
Halpern, 1970, The effect of multiple responses and certainty estimates on the integration of visual information, Perception and Psychophysics, 7, 129, 10.3758/BF03208640
Hammond, 1955, Probabilistic functioning and the clinical method, Psychological Review, 62, 255, 10.1037/h0046845
Hammond, 1965, New directions in research in conflict resolution, Journal of Social Issues, 21, 44, 10.1111/j.1540-4560.1965.tb00505.x
Hammond, 1966, Probabilistic functionalism: Egon Brunswik's integration of the history, theory and method of psychology
Hammond, 1970, Quasi-rationality, quarrels, and new conceptions of feedback
Hammond, K. R., & Brehmer, B. The quasi-rational nature of quarrels about policy. In J. Hellmuth (Ed.), Cognitive Studies, Vol. 2, Deficits in Cognition. New York: Brunner/Mazel, Inc., in press.
Hammond, 1964, Analyzing the components of clinical inference, Psychological Review, 71, 438, 10.1037/h0040736
Hammond, 1967, Clinical inference in nursing: Revising judgments, Nursing Research, 16, 36, 10.1097/00006199-196701610-00008
Hammond, 1965, Cognitive dependence on linear and nonlinear cues, Psychological Review, 72, 215, 10.1037/h0021798
Hammond, 1966, Cognitive conflict between persons: Application of the “Lens Model” paradigm, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 2, 343, 10.1016/0022-1031(66)90027-8
Hammond, 1966, A research paradigm for the study of interpersonal learning, Psychological Bulletin, 65, 221, 10.1037/h0023103
Hayes, 1964, Human data processing limits in decision making
Hayes, 1968, Strategies in judgmental research
Hays, 1963
Hendrick, 1968, Averaging vs. summation in impression formation, Perceptual and Motor Skills, 27, 1295, 10.2466/pms.1968.27.3f.1295
Hendrick, 1970, Effects of varying trait inconsistency and response requirements on the primacy effect in impression formation, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 15, 158, 10.1037/h0029203
Hendrick, 1970, Number averaging behavior: A primacy effect, Psychonomic Science, 19, 121, 10.3758/BF03337452
Himmelfarb, 1970, The impact of neutral information about a person
Himmelfarr, 1969, Forming impressions of social class: Two tests of an averaging model, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 12, 38, 10.1037/h0027359
Hoepfl, 1970, A study of self-explicated utility models, Behavioral Science, 15, 408, 10.1002/bs.3830150503
Hoffman, 1960, The paramorphic representation of clinical judgment, Psychological Bulletin, 47, 116, 10.1037/h0047807
Hoffman, 1968, Cue-consistency and configurality in human judgment
Hoffman, 1961, A study of the effects of varying amounts of predictor information on judgment, Oregon Research Institute Research Bulletin
Hoffman, 1968, An analysis-of-variance model for the assessment of configural cue utilization in clinical judgment, Psychological Bulletin, 69, 338, 10.1037/h0025665
Howell, 1967, Some principles for the design of decision systems: A review of six years of research on a command-control system simulation, AMRL-TR-67-136
Huber, 1969, A study of subjective evaluation models, Behavioral Science, 14, 483, 10.1002/bs.3830140606
Hursch, 1964, Some methodological considerations in multiple cue probability studies, Psychological Review, 71, 42, 10.1037/h0041729
Hurst, 1967, Heuristic scheduling in a woolen mill, Management Science, 14, B-182, 10.1287/mnsc.14.4.B182
Jones, 1967, Parametric production planning, Management Science, 13, 843, 10.1287/mnsc.13.11.843
Jones, M. R. From probability learning to sequential processing: A critical review. Psychological Bulletin, in press.
Kahneman, 1970, Subjective probability: A judgment of representativeness, Oregon Research Institute Research Bulletin, Vol. 10
Kaplan, 1966, Studies in probabilistic information processing, IEEE Transactions on Human Factors in Electronics, 7, 49, 10.1109/THFE.1966.231983
Kates, 1962, Hazard and choice perception in flood plain management
Katona, 1951
Kerrick, 1958, The effect of relevant and non-relevant sources on attitude change, Journal of Social Psychology, 47, 15, 10.1080/00224545.1958.9714338
Klahr, 1969, Decision making in a complex environment: The use of similarity judgments to predict preferences, Management Science, 15, 595, 10.1287/mnsc.15.11.595
Kleinmuntz, 1968, The processing of clinical information by man and machine
Knox, 1962, Effects of variation of profile format on intelligence and sociability judgments, Journal of Applied Psychology, 46, 14, 10.1037/h0047107
Koford, 1966, The use of an adaptive threshold element to design a linear optimal pattern classifier, IEEE Transactions on Information Theory, Vol. IT-12, 42, 10.1109/TIT.1966.1053856
Kort, 1968, A nonlinear model for the analysis of judicial decisions, The American Political Science Review, 62, 546, 10.2307/1952948
Krantz, 1971, Conjoint measurement analysis of composition rules in psychology, Psychological Review, 78, 151, 10.1037/h0030637
Kriz, 1967, Der Likelihood Quotient zur erfassung des subjektiven Signifikanzniveaus
Kunreuther, 1969, Extensions of Bowman's theory on managerial decision-making, Management Science, 15, 415, 10.1287/mnsc.15.8.B415
Lampel, 1968, Combining visual and verbal information in an impression-formation task, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9, 1, 10.1037/h0025716
Ledley, 1959, Reasoning foundations of medical diagnosis, Science, 130, 9, 10.1126/science.130.3366.9
Lee, 1962, An investigation of clinical judgment: A study in method, Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 64, 272, 10.1037/h0042055
Levin, 1969, Sequential effects in impression formation with binary intermittent responding, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 79, 283, 10.1037/h0027045
Lichtenstein, 1968, The importance of the data-generating model in probability estimation, Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 3, 62, 10.1016/0030-5073(68)90027-5
Lindblom, 1964, The science of “muddling through”
Loeb, 1965
Luce, 1964, Simultaneous conjoint measurement: A new type of fundamental measurement, Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 1, 1, 10.1016/0022-2496(64)90015-X
Luchins, 1957, Experimental attempts to minimize the impact of first impressions
Luchins, 1958, Definitiveness of impression and primacy-recency in communications, The Journal of Social Psychology, 48, 275, 10.1080/00224545.1958.9919292
Lusted, 1968
Madden, 1963, An application to job evaluation of a policy-capturing model for analyzing individual and group judgment, 6570th Personnel Research Laboratory Aerospace Medical Division Air Force Systems Command, PRL-TDR-63-15
Magnusson, 1969, Cue relevance and feedback in a clinical prediction task, 10.1037/e573002009-001
Maguire, 1968, Component profile analysis (COPAN)-An alternative to PROF, Educational and Psychological Measurement, 28, 1021, 10.1177/001316446802800402
Manis, 1966, The evaluation of complex social stimuli, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 4, 404, 10.1037/h0023039
Martin, 1969, Data conflict in a multinomial decision task, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 82, 4, 10.1037/h0028051
Martin, 1969, Feedback and response mode in performing a Bayesian decision task, Journal of Applied Psychology, 53, 413, 10.1037/h0028052
Martin, 1957, The nature of clinical judgment
McEachern, 1969, A system for computer-aided probation decision-making, Journal of Research on Crime and Delinquency, 6, 184, 10.1177/002242786900600207
Meehl, 1954
Miller, 1956, The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information, Psychological Review, 63, 81, 10.1037/h0043158
Miller, M. J., & Sarafino, E. The effects of intercorrelated cues on multiple probability learning. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, in press.
Morrison, 1962, Effects of context on relative judgments of area
Naylor, 1968, Intuitive inference strategies in interval learning tasks as a function of validity magnitude and sign, Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 3, 378, 10.1016/0030-5073(68)90016-0
Naylor, 1968, The influence of cue redundancy upon the human inference process for tasks of varying degrees of predictability, Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 3, 47, 10.1016/0030-5073(68)90026-3
Naylor, 1965, The use of simulated stimuli and the “JAN” technique to capture and cluster the policies of raters, Educational and Psychological Measurement, 25, 969, 10.1177/001316446502500403
Newell, 1958, Elements of a theory of human problem solving, Psychological Review, 65, 151, 10.1037/h0048495
Newton, 1965, Judgment and feedback in a quasi-clinical situation, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1, 336, 10.1037/h0021808
Oden, G. C., & Anderson, N. H. Differential weighting in integration theory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, in press.
Osgood, 1955, The principle of congruity in the prediction of attitude change, Psychological Review, 62, 42, 10.1037/h0048153
Oskamp, 1962, How clinicians make decisions from the MMPI: An empirical study
Oskamp, 1965, Overconfidence in case-study judgments, Journal of Consulting Psychology, 29, 261, 10.1037/h0022125
Pankoff, 1968, Bayesian synthesis of clinical and statistical prediction, Psychological Bulletin, 70, 762, 10.1037/h0026831
Parducci, 1968, Stimulus averaging and the context for judgment, Perception and Psychophysics, 3, 145, 10.3758/BF03212782
Peterson, 1968, Aggregating information about signals and noise, 123
Peterson, 1967, Man as an intuitive statistician, Psychological Bulletin, 68, 29, 10.1037/h0024722
Peterson, 1967, A primacy effect in subjective probability revision, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 73, 61, 10.1037/h0024139
Peterson, 1968, Sampling distributions and probability revisions, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 76, 236, 10.1037/h0025427
Peterson, 1965, Multiple probability learning with shifting cue weights, American Journal of Psychology, 78, 660, 10.2307/1420932
Peterson, 1965, Optimal responding in multiple-cue probability learning, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70, 270, 10.1037/h0022261
Peterson, 1965, Sensitivity of subjective probability revision, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70, 117, 10.1037/h0022023
Peterson, 1966, Revision of continuous subjective probability distributions, IEEE Transactions on Human Factors in Electronics, HFE-7, 19, 10.1109/THFE.1966.231979
Peterson, 1965, Sample size and the revision of subjective probabilities, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69, 522, 10.1037/h0021720
Peterson, 1968, Intuitive statistical inferences about diffuse hypotheses, Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 3, 1, 10.1016/0030-5073(68)90023-8
Phillips, 1966, Some components of probabilistic inference
Phillips, 1966, Conservatism in a simple probability inference task, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72, 346, 10.1037/h0023653
Phillips, 1966, Conservatism in complex probabilistic inference, IEEE Transactions on Human Factors in Electronics, HFE-7, 7, 10.1109/THFE.1966.231978
Pitz, 1966, The sequential judgment of proportion, Psychonomic Science, 4, 397, 10.3758/BF03342357
Pitz, 1967, Sample size, likelihood, and confidence in a decision, Psychonomic Science, 8, 257, 10.3758/BF03331649
Pitz, 1969, An inertia effect (resistance to change) in the revision of opinion, Canadian Journal of Psychology, 23, 24, 10.1037/h0082790
Pitz, 1969, The influence of prior probabilities on information seeking and decision making, Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 4, 213, 10.1016/0030-5073(69)90006-3
Pitz, 1969, Use of response times to evaluate strategies of information seeking, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 80, 553, 10.1037/h0027487
Pitz, 1967, Optimal behavior in a decision-making task as a function of instructions and payoffs, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 73, 549, 10.1037/h0024345
Pitz, 1967, Sequential effects in the revision of subjective probabilities, Canadian Journal of Psychology, 21, 381, 10.1037/h0082998
Pitz, 1968, Payoff effects in sequential decision-making, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 77, 249, 10.1037/h0025802
Platt, 1964, Strong inference, Science, 146, 347, 10.1126/science.146.3642.347
Podell, 1962, The impression as a quantitative concept, American Psychologist, 17, 308
Podell, 1963, Quantitative connotation of a concept, Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67, 509, 10.1037/h0041513
Pollack, 1964, Action selection and the Yntema-Torgerson worth function
Pruitt, 1961, Informational requirements in making decisions, American Journal of Psychology, 74, 433, 10.2307/1419750
Raiffa, 1961
Rapoport, 1970
Rappoport, 1965, Interpersonal conflict in cooperative and uncertain situations, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 1, 323, 10.1016/0022-1031(65)90012-0
Rhine, 1968, Test of models and impression formation
Roby, 1967, Belief states and sequential evidence, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 75, 236, 10.1037/h0024996
Rodwan, 1964, The discriminant-function as a model for perception, American Journal of Psychology, 26, 380, 10.2307/1421008
Rorer, 1967, Configural judgments revealed, 2, 195
Rosenberg, 1968, Mathematical models of social behavior, 1, 186
Rosenkrantz, 1965, Some factors influencing the assimilation of disparate information in impression formation, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2, 397, 10.1037/h0022286
Russell, 1969, Losses from natural hazards
Sarbin, 1942, A contribution to the study of actuarial and individual methods of prediction, American Journal of Sociology, 48, 593, 10.1086/219248
Sarbin, 1966, The immediacy postulate in the light of modern cognitive psychology
Savage, 1954
Schenck, 1968, A cautionary note concerning the use of regression analysis for capturing the strategies of people, Educational and Psychological Measurement, 28, 3, 10.1177/001316446802800101
Schlaifer, 1959
Schmidt, 1969, Personality impression formation as a function of relatedness of information and length of set, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 12, 6, 10.1037/h0027363
Schum, 1966, Inferences on the basis of conditionally nonindependent data, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72, 401, 10.1037/h0023651
Schum, 1966, Prior uncertainty and amount of diagnostic evidence as variables in a probabilistic inference task, Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 1, 31, 10.1016/0030-5073(66)90004-3
Schum, 1967, Concerning the evaluation and aggregation of probabilistic evidence by man-machine systems
Schum, 1968, Behavioral decision theory and man-machine systems
Schum, 1969, Concerning the simulation of diagnostic systems which process complex probabilistic evidence sets, Aerospace Medical Research Laboratory, Wright-Patterson Air Force Base, Ohio, Technical Report 69-10
Schum, 1967, Subjective probability revisions under several cost-payoff arrangements, Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 2, 84, 10.1016/0030-5073(67)90012-8
Schum, 1966, Research on a simulated Bayesian information-processing system, IEEE Transactions on Human Factors in Electronics, HFE-7, 37, 10.1109/THFE.1966.231982
Schum, 1968, Human processing of inconclusive evidence from multinomial probability distributions, Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 3, 353, 10.1016/0030-5073(68)90014-7
Schum, 1969, Aided human processing of inconclusive evidence in diagnostic systems: A summary of experimental evaluations
Shanteau, 1970, An additive model for sequential decision making, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 85, 181, 10.1037/h0029552
Shanteau, 1969, Test of a conflict model for preference judgment, Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 6, 312, 10.1016/0022-2496(69)90009-1
Sheets, C., & Miller, M. J. The effect of cue-criterion function form on multiple cue probability learning. American Journal of Psychology, in press.
Shepard, 1964, On subjectively optimum selection among multiattribute alternatives
Sidowski, 1967, Judgments of city-occupation combinations, Psychonomic Science, 7, 279, 10.3758/BF03328559
Simon, 1956, Rational choice and the structure of the environment, Psychological Review, 63, 129, 10.1037/h0042769
Simon, 1969
Slovic, 1966, Cue consistency and cue utilization in judgment, American Journal of Psychology, 79, 427, 10.2307/1420883
Slovic, 1969, Analyzing the expert judge: A descriptive study of a stockbroker's decision processes, Journal of Applied Psychology, 53, 255, 10.1037/h0027773
Slovic, P., Fleissner, D., & Bauman, W. S. Quantitative analysis of investment decisions. Journal of Business, in press.
Slovic, 1968, The relative importance of probabilities and payoffs in risk taking, Journal of Experimental Psychology Monograph Supplement, 78, 10.1037/h0026468
Slovic, 1971, Analyzing the use of diagnostic signs, Investigative Radiology., 6, 18, 10.1097/00004424-197101000-00003
Smedslund, 1955
Smith, 1968
Stewart, 1965, Effect of continuous responding on the order effect in personality impression formation, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1, 161, 10.1037/h0021641
Summers, 1967, Rule versus cue learning in multiple probability tasks, 2, 43
Summers, 1968, Conflict, compromise, and belief change in a decision-making task, Journal of Conflict Resolution, 12, 215, 10.1177/002200276801200206
Summers, 1969, Adaptation to change in multiple probability tasks, American Journal of Psychology, 82, 235, 10.2307/1421248
Summers, 1966, Inference behavior in multiple-cue tasks involving both linear and nonlinear relations, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71, 751, 10.1037/h0023122
Summers, 1968, Regression models of foreign policy judgments, 3, 195
Summers, 1962, The learning of responses to multiple weighted cues, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 29, 10.1037/h0044546
Summers, 1969, Judgments based on different functional relationships between interacting cues and a criterion, American Journal of Psychology, 82, 203, 10.2307/1421244
1964
Swets, 1967, Deferred decision in human signal detection: A preliminary experiment, Perception and Psychophysics, 2, 15, 10.3758/BF03210060
Toda, 1963, Measurement of subjective probability distribution
Todd, 1965, Differential feedback in two multiple-cue probability learning tasks, Behavioral Science, 10, 429, 10.1002/bs.3830100406
Tucker, 1964, A suggested alternative formulation in the development of Hursch, Hammond, & Hursch, and by Hammond, Hursch, and Todd, Psychological Review, 71, 528, 10.1037/h0047061
Tversky, 1967, Additivity, utility, and subjective probability, Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 4, 175, 10.1016/0022-2496(67)90049-1
Tversky, 1967, A general theory of polynomial conjoint measurement, Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 4, 1, 10.1016/0022-2496(67)90039-9
Tversky, 1967, Utility theory and additivity analysis of risky choices, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 75, 27, 10.1037/h0024915
Tversky, 1969, Intransitivity of preferences, Psychological Review, 76, 31, 10.1037/h0026750
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. The belief in the law of small numbers. Psychological Bulletin, in press.
Tversky, 1971, The judgment of probability by retrieval and construction of instances, Oregon Research Institute Research Bulletin, Vol. 11
Tversky, 1969, Similarity of schematic faces: A test of interdimensional additivity, Perception & Psychophysics, 5, 124, 10.3758/BF03210535
Uhl, 1963, Learning of interval concepts. I. Effects of differences in stimulus weights, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 264, 10.1037/h0043611
Uhl, 1958, Contagion effects and the stability of judgment
Ulehla, 1966, Optimality of perceptual decision criteria, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71, 564, 10.1037/h0023007
Ulehla, 1967, Integration of conceptual information, Psychonomic Science, 8, 223, 10.3758/BF03331632
Ulmer, 1969, The discriminant function and a theoretical context for its use in estimating the votes of judges
Vlek, 1965, The use of probabilistic information in decision making
Vlek, 1967, Subjective likelihoods in posterior probability estimation
Vlek, 1967, Subjective likelihood functions and variations in the accuracy of probabilistic information processing
von Neumann, 1947
Wald, 1947
Wallsten, 1968, Failure of predictions from subjectively expected utility theory in a Bayesian decision task, Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 3, 239, 10.1016/0030-5073(68)90008-1
Wallsten, 1970, The likelihood-ratio principle and conjoint measurement
Ward, 1963, Teaching a digital computer to assist in making decisions
Weiss, 1969, Subjective averaging of length with serial presentation, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 82, 52, 10.1037/h0028028
Weiss, 1963, Scale judgments of triplets of opinion statements, Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 66, 471, 10.1037/h0045735
Wendt, 1969, Value of information for decisions, Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 6, 430, 10.1016/0022-2496(69)90015-7
Wheeler, 1968, Subjective sampling distributions and conservatism, Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 3, 36, 10.1016/0030-5073(68)90025-1
Wherry, 1966, Comparison of two approaches-JAN and PROF-for capturing rater strategies, Educational and Psychological Measurement, 26, 267, 10.1177/001316446602600202
White, 1966, Optimal flood damage management: Retrospect and prospect
Wiggins, N. Multivariate models for cue-utilization: An individual differences approach. In L. Rappoport and D. Summers (Eds.), Human judgment and social interaction. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, in press.
Wiggins, 1968, Three models of clinical judgment, Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 73, 70, 10.1037/h0025445
Williams, 1970, A judgment analysis program for clustering similar judgmental systems, Educational and Psychological Measurement, 30, 171, 10.1177/001316447003000126
Willis, 1960, Stimulus pooling and social perception, Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 60, 365, 10.1037/h0048912
Winkler, 1968, “Good” probability assessors, Journal of Applied Meteorology, 7, 751, 10.1175/1520-0450(1968)007<0751:PA>2.0.CO;2
Wohlstetter, 1962
Wyer, 1968, The effects of information redundancy on evaluations of social stimuli, Psychonomic Science, 13, 245, 10.3758/BF03342505
Wyer, 1970, Information redundancy, inconsistency, and novelty and their role in impression formation, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 6, 111, 10.1016/0022-1031(70)90079-X
Wyer, 1969, Context effects in impression formation, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 12, 22, 10.1037/h0027365
Yntema, 1964, Telling a computer how to evaluate alternatives as one would evaluate them himself
Yntema, 1961, Man-computer cooperation in decisions requiring common sense, IRE Transactions of the Professional Group on Human Factors in Electronics, HFE 2, 20, 10.1109/THFE2.1961.4503294