Becoming Better Learners, Becoming Better Teachers: Augmenting Learning via Cognitive and Motivational Theories

Veronica X. Yan1, Brendan A. Schuetze2, Stephany Duany Rea1
1Department of Educational Psychology, The University of Texas at Austin, 1912 Speedway M/S D5800, Austin, TX, 78712, USA
2Department of Educational Sciences, University of Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany

Tóm tắt

Từ khóa


Tài liệu tham khảo

Agarwal, P. K. (2019). Retrieval practice & Bloom’s taxonomy: Do students need fact knowledge before higher order learning? Journal of Educational Psychology, 111, 189–209. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000282

Agarwal, P. K., Nunes, L. D., & Blunt, J. R. (2021). Retrieval practice consistently benefits student learning: A systematic review of applied research in schools and classrooms. Educational Psychology Review, 33(4), 1409–1453. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-021-09595-9

Ausubel, D. P. (1968). Educational psychology: A cognitive view. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.

Baddeley, A. (2006). Working memory. In Working memory and education (pp. 1–31). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-012554465-8/50003-X

Begg, I., Duft, S., Lalonde, P., Melnick, R., & Sanvito, J. (1989). Memory predictions are based on ease of processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 610–632. https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(89)90016-8

Biwer, F., oude Egbrink, M. G. A., Aalten, P., & de Bruin, A. B. H. (2020). Fostering effective learning strategies in higher education – A mixed-methods study. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 9(2), 186–203. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2020.03.004

Bjork, R. A. (2018). Being suspicious of the sense of ease and undeterred by the sense of difficulty: Looking back at Schmidt and Bjork (1992). Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13(2), 146–148. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691617690642

Bjork, R. A., Dunlosky, J., & Kornell, N. (2013). Self-regulated learning: Beliefs, techniques, and illusions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 417–444. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03194055

Blasiman, R. N., Dunlosky, J., & Rawson, K. A. (2017). The what, how much, and when of study strategies: Comparing intended versus actual study behaviour. Memory, 25(6), 784–792. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2016.1221974

Blunt, J. R., & Karpicke, J. D. (2014). Learning with retrieval-based concept mapping. Journal of Educational Psychology, 106, 849–858. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035934

Brown, P. C., Roediger, H. L. R., III., & McDaniel, M. A. (2014). Make it stick: The science of successful learning. Harvard University Press.

Brown, T., Mann, B., Ryder, N., Subbiah, M., Kaplan, J. D., Dhariwal, P., Neelakantan, A., Shyam, P., Sastry, G., Askell, A., et al. (2020). Language models are few-shot learners. Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems, 33, 1877–1901.

Brunmair, M., & Richter, T. (2019). Similarity matters: A meta-analysis of interleaved learning and its moderators. Psychological Bulletin, 145(11), 1029–1052. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000209

Bryant, J., Child, F., Dorn, E., Espinosa, J., Hall, S., Kola-Oyeneyin, T., Lim, C., Panier, F., Sarakatsannis, J., Schmautzer, D., Ungur, S., & Woord, B. (2022). How COVID-19 caused a global learning crisis. McKinsey & Company. Retrieved June 6, 2023, from https://www.mckinsey.com/industries/education/our-insights/how-covid-19-caused-a-global-learning-crisis

Bugg, J. M., DeLosh, E. L., & McDaniel, M. A. (2008). Improving students’ study habits by demonstrating the mnemonic benefits of semantic processing. Teaching of Psychology, 35, 96–98. https://doi.org/10.1080/00986280801977519

Carpenter, S. K. (2012). Testing enhances the transfer of learning. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(5), 279–283. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721412452728

Carpenter, S. K. (2017). Spacing effects on learning and memory. In J. H. Byrne (Ed.), Learning and Memory: A Comprehensive Reference (Second Edition) (pp. 465–485). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-809324-5.21054-7

Carpenter, S. K., Pan, S. C., & Butler, A. C. (2022). The science of effective learning with spacing and retrieval practice. Nature Reviews Psychology, 1(9), 496–511. https://doi.org/10.1038/s44159-022-00089-1

Carroll, J. B. (1962). The prediction of success in intensive foreign language training. In R. Glaser (Ed.), Training Research and Education (pp. 87–136). University of Pittsburgh Press.

Carroll, J. B. (1963). A model of school learning. Teachers College Record, 64(8), 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1177/016146816306400801

Carvalho, P. F., & Goldstone, R. L. (2015). The benefits of interleaved and blocked study: Different tasks benefit from different schedules of study. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22(1), 281–288. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-014-0676-4

Carvalho, P. F., & Goldstone, R. L. (2019). When does interleaving practice improve learning? In J. Dunlosky & K. A. Rawson (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of cognition and education (pp. 411–436). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108235631.017

Cepeda, N. J., Pashler, H., Vul, E., Wixted, J. T., & Rohrer, D. (2006). Distributed practice in verbal recall tasks: A review and quantitative synthesis. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 354–380. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.132.3.354

Cepeda, N. J., Vul, E., Rohrer, D., Wixted, J. T., & Pashler, H. (2008). Spacing effects in learning: A temporal ridgeline of optimal retention. Psychological Science, 19(11), 1095–1102. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02209.x

Christodoulou, P. (2022). Learning is remembering. Save All. Retrieved June 6, 2023, from https://saveall.ai/blog/learning-is-remembering

Cowan, N. (2012). Working memory capacity (1st ed.). Psychology Press. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203342398

Deslauriers, L., Schelew, E., & Wieman, C. (2011). Improved learning in a large-enrollment physics class. Science, 332(6031), 862–864. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1201783

Dorn, E., Hancock, B., Sarakatsannis, J., & Viruleg, E. (2021). COVID-19 and education: The lingering effects of unfinished learning. McKinsey & Company. Retrieved June 6, 2023, from https://www.mckinsey.com/industries/education/our-insights/covid-19-and-education-the-lingering-effects-of-unfinished-learning

Dunlosky, J., & Metcalfe, J. (2008). Metacognition. SAGE Publications.

Dunlosky, J., & Rawson, K. A. (2015). Practice tests, spaced practice, and successive relearning: Tips for classroom use and for guiding students’ learning. Scholarship of Teaching and Learning in Psychology, 1, 72–78. https://doi.org/10.1037/stl0000024

Elliot, A. J., & Murayama, K. (2008). On the measurement of achievement goals: Critique, illustration, and application. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(3), 613–628. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.100.3.613

Foster, N. L., Mueller, M. L., Was, C., Rawson, K. A., & Dunlosky, J. (2019). Why does interleaving improve math learning? The contributions of discriminative contrast and distributed practice. Memory & Cognition, 47(6), 1088–1101. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-019-00918-4

GEEAP. (2020). Cost-effective approaches to improve global learning: What does recent evidence tell us are “Smart Buys” for improving learning in low- and middle-income countries? The World Bank. Retrieved June 6, 2023, from https://documents1.worldbank.org/curated/en/719211603835247448/pdf/Cost-Effective-Approaches-to-Improve-Global-Learning-What-Does-Recent-Evidence-Tell-Us-Are-Smart-Buys-for-Improving-Learning-in-Low-and-Middle-Income-Countries.pdf

Goldberg, S. B. (2021). Education in a pandemic: The disparate impacts of COVID-19 on America’s students. U.S. Department of Education, Office for Civil Rights. Retrieved June 6, 2023, from https://www2.ed.gov/about/offices/list/ocr/docs/20210608-impacts-of-covid19.pdf

Gollwitzer, P. M., & Sheeran, P. (2006). Implementation intentions and goal achievement: A meta-analysis of effects and processes. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 69–119. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(06)38002-1

Hattie, J. (2012). Visible learning for teachers: Maximizing impact on learning. Routledge.

Hopkins, R. F., Lyle, K. B., Hieb, J. L., & Ralston, P. A. (2016). Spaced retrieval practice increases college students’ short-and long-term retention of mathematics knowledge. Educational Psychology Review, 28(4), 853–873.

Kalyuga, S., Ayres, P., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2003). The expertise reversal effect. Educational Psychologist, 38(1), 23–31. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15326985EP3801_4

Kang, S. H. K. (2016). Spaced repetition promotes efficient and effective learning: Policy implications for instruction. Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 3(1), 12–19. https://doi.org/10.1177/2372732215624708

Karpicke, J. D., & Blunt, J. R. (2011). Retrieval practice produces more learning than elaborative studying with concept mapping. Science, 331(6018), 772–775. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1199327

Kirk-Johnson, A., Galla, B. M., & Fraundorf, S. H. (2019). Perceiving effort as poor learning: The misinterpreted-effort hypothesis of how experienced effort and perceived learning relate to study strategy choice. Cognitive Psychology, 115, 101237. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2019.101237

Kirschner, P. A., Sweller, J., & Clark, R. E. (2006). Why minimal guidance during instruction does not work: An analysis of the failure of constructivist, discovery, problem-based, experiential, and inquiry-based teaching. Educational Psychologist, 41(2), 75–86. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep4102_1

Koriat, A. (2008). Easy comes, easy goes? The link between learning and remembering and its exploitation in metacognition. Memory & Cognition, 36(2), 416–428. https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.36.2.416

Kornell, N., & Bjork, R. A. (2007). The promise and perils of self-regulated study. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(2), 219–224. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03194055

Kornell, N., & Bjork, R. A. (2008). Learning concepts and categories: Is spacing the “enemy of induction”? Psychological Science, 19(6), 585–592. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02127.x

Kraft, M. A. (2020). Interpreting effect sizes of education interventions. Educational Researcher, 49(4), 241–253. https://doi.org/10.3102/0013189X20912798

Kuhfeld, M., Soland, J., Lewis, K., & Morton, E. (2022, March 3). The pandemic has had devastating impacts on learning. What will it take to help students catch up? Brookings Institute: The Brown Center Chalkboard. Retrieved June 6, 2023, from https://www.brookings.edu/blog/brown-center-chalkboard/2022/03/03/the-pandemic-has-had-devastating-impacts-on-learning-what-will-it-take-to-help-students-catch-up/

Kumar, K. P., & Geethakumari, G. (2014). Detecting misinformation in online social networks using cognitive psychology. Human-Centric Computing and Information Sciences, 4(1), 1–22. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13673-014-0014-x

Lin, C., McDaniel, M. A., & Miyatsu, T. (2018). Effects of flashcards on learning authentic materials: The role of detailed versus conceptual flashcards and individual differences in structure-building ability. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 7, 529–539. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0101829

Macaluso, J. A., Beuford, R. R., & Fraundorf, S. H. (2022). Familiar strategies feel fluent: The role of study strategy familiarity in the misinterpreted-effort model of self-regulated Learning. Journal of Intelligence, 10(4), 4. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10040083

McCabe, J. (2011). Metacognitive awareness of learning strategies in undergraduates. Memory & Cognition, 39(3), 462–476. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-010-0035-2

McCabe, J. A. (2018). What learning strategies do academic support centers recommend to undergraduates? Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 7(1), 143–153. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2017.10.002

McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (2020). Training learning strategies to promote self-regulation and transfer: The knowledge, belief, commitment, and planning framework. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 15(6), 1363–1381. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691620920723

Morehead, K., Rhodes, M. G., & DeLozier, S. (2016). Instructor and student knowledge of study strategies. Memory, 24(2), 257–271. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2014.1001992

Muenks, K., Yan, V. X., Woodward, N. R., & Frey, S. E. (2021). Elaborative learning practices are associated with perceived faculty growth mindset in undergraduate science classrooms. Learning and Individual Differences, 92, 102088. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2021.102088

Nemeth, L., Werker, K., Arend, J., Vogel, S., & Lipowsky, F. (2019). Interleaved learning in elementary school mathematics: Effects on the flexible and adaptive use of subtraction strategies. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 86. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00086

O’Day, G. M., & Karpicke, J. D. (2021). Comparing and combining retrieval practice and concept mapping. Journal of Educational Psychology, 113(5), 986–997. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000486

Pan, S. C., & Bjork, R. A. (in press). Acquiring an accurate mental model of human learning: Toward an owner’s manual. In A. Wagner & M. Kahana (Eds.), Oxford Handbook of Memory, Volume II: Applications. Retrieved June 6, 2023, from https://www.oxfordhandbooks.com/

Rea, S. D., Wang, L., Muenks, K., & Yan, V. X. (2022). Students can (mostly) recognize effective learning, so why do they not do it? Journal of Intelligence, 10(4), 4. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10040127

Reber, R., & Unkelbach, C. (2010). The epistemic status of processing fluency as source for judgments of truth. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 1(4), 563–581. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-010-0039-7

Reder, L. M., Liu, X. L., Keinath, A., & Popov, V. (2016). Building knowledge requires bricks, not sand: The critical role of familiar constituents in learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23(1), 271–277. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-015-0889-1

Risko, E. F., & Dunn, T. L. (2015). Storing information in-the-world: Metacognition and cognitive offloading in a short-term memory task. Consciousness and Cognition, 36, 61–74. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2015.05.014

Risko, E. F., & Gilbert, S. J. (2016). Cognitive offloading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(9), 676–688. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2016.07.002

Rivers, M. L. (2021). Metacognition about practice testing: A review of learners’ beliefs, monitoring, and control of test-enhanced learning. Educational Psychology Review, 33(3), 823–862. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-020-09578-2

Roediger, H. L., III., & Butler, A. C. (2011). The critical role of retrieval practice in long-term retention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(1), 20–27. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.09.003

Rohrer, D. (2012). Interleaving helps students distinguish among similar concepts. Educational Psychology Review, 24(3), 355–367. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-012-9201-3

Rohrer, D., Dedrick, R. F., & Hartwig, M. K. (2020a). The scarcity of interleaved practice in mathematics textbooks. Educational Psychology Review, 32(3), 873–883. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-020-09516-2

Rohrer, D., Dedrick, R. F., Hartwig, M. K., & Cheung, C.-N. (2020b). A randomized controlled trial of interleaved mathematics practice. Journal of Educational Psychology, 112, 40–52. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000367

Samani, J., & Pan, S. C. (2021). Interleaved practice enhances memory and problem-solving ability in undergraduate physics. NPJ Science of Learning, 6(1), 1–11.

Sana, F., & Yan, V. X. (2022). Interleaving retrieval practice promotes science learning. Psychological Science, 33(5), 782–788. https://doi.org/10.1177/09567976211057507

Santens, S. (2017). Stop teaching students what to think. Teach them how to think. Education Week. Retrieved June 6, 2023, from https://www.edweek.org/education/opinion-stop-teaching-students-what-to-think-teach-them-how-to-think/2017/09

Schell, J. A., & Butler, A. C. (2018). Insights from the science of learning can inform evidence-based implementation of peer instruction. Frontiers in Education. https://doi.org/10.3389/feduc.2018.00033

Schraw, G., & Moshman, D. (1995). Metacognitive theories. Educational Psychology Review, 7(4), 351–371. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02212307

Snyder, L. G., & Snyder, M. J. (2008). Teaching critical thinking and problem solving skills. The Journal of Research in Business Education, 50(2), 90.

Soderstrom, N. C., & Bjork, R. A. (2015). Learning versus performance: An integrative review. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(2), 176–199. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691615569000

Storm, B. C., & Soares, J. S. (in press). Memory in the digital age. In A. Wagner & M. Kahana (Eds.), Oxford Handbook of Memory, Volume II: Applications. Retrieved June 6, 2023, from https://www.oxfordhandbooks.com/

Sumeracki, M. A., & Castillo, J. (2022). Covert and overt retrieval practice in the classroom. Translational Issues in Psychological Science, 8, 282–293. https://doi.org/10.1037/tps0000332

Sun, K. L. (2019). The mindset disconnect in mathematics teaching: A qualitative analysis of classroom instruction. The Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 56, 100706. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jmathb.2019.04.005

Undorf, M., & Erdfelder, E. (2011). Judgments of learning reflect encoding fluency: Conclusive evidence for the ease-of-processing hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37, 1264–1269. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023719

Willingham, D. T. (2019). The digital expansion of the mind gone wrong in education. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 8(1), 20–24. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2018.12.001

Wissman, K. T., Rawson, K. A., & Pyc, M. A. (2012). How and when do students use flashcards? Memory, 20(6), 568–579. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2012.687052

Yan, V. X., & Sana, F. (2021a). Does the interleaving effect extend to unrelated concepts? Learners’ beliefs versus empirical evidence. Journal of Educational Psychology, 113(1), 125–137. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000470

Yan, V. X., & Sana, F. (2021b). The robustness of the interleaving benefit. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 10(4), 589–602. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2021.05.002

Yan, V. X., Bjork, E. L., & Bjork, R. A. (2016). On the difficulty of mending metacognitive illusions: A priori theories, fluency effects, and misattributions of the interleaving benefit. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(7), 918–933. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000177

Yan, V. X., Soderstrom, N. C., Seneviratna, G. S., Bjork, E. L., & Bjork, R. A. (2017). How should exemplars be sequenced in inductive learning? Empirical evidence versus learners’ opinions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 23, 403–416. https://doi.org/10.1037/xap0000139

Yan, V. X., Yu, Y., Garcia, M. A., & Bjork, R. A. (2014). Why does guessing incorrectly enhance, rather than impair, retention? Memory & Cognition, 42(8), 1373–1383. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-014-0454-6

Yang, C., Luo, L., Vadillo, M. A., Yu, R., & Shanks, D. R. (2021). Testing (quizzing) boosts classroom learning: A systematic and meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 147(4), 399–435. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000309

Zeeb, H., Ostertag, J., & Renkl, A. (2020). Towards a growth mindset culture in the classroom: Implementation of a lesson-integrated mindset training. Education Research International. https://doi.org/10.1155/2020/8067619

Zepeda, C. D., Martin, R. S., & Butler, A. C. (2020). Motivational strategies to engage learners in desirable difficulties. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 9(4), 468–474. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2020.08.007

Zung, I., Imundo, M. N., & Pan, S. C. (2022). How do college students use digital flashcards during self-regulated learning? Memory, 30(8), 923–941. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2022.2058553