A Negative Mood Induction Procedure with Efficacy Across Repeated Administrations in Women
Tóm tắt
Although several experimental techniques are effective in inducing negative mood, none has established efficacy over repeated testing. The lack of mood induction tools validated across repeated administrations impedes study of emotional changes in the same individual over time. A brief, sad mood induction combining music and an autobiographical memory was administered to 17 women on 6 occasions (two 3-day sets over a 2-week period of time). Mood was measured before and after the induction procedure. Repeated measures ANOVAs showed that the induction produced a large increase in POMS depression (d = 1.48) that recurred consistently across all testing days. A brief negative mood induction combining music and autobiographical memory appears suitable for use in studies that involve repeated administrations.
Tài liệu tham khảo
Boyle, G. J. (1987). A cross-validation of the factor structure of the Profile of Mood States: Were the factors correctly identified in the first instance? Psychological Reports, 60(2), 343-354.
Brewer, D., Doughtie, E. B., & Lubin, B. (1980). Induction of mood and mood shift. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 36, 215-226.
Buchwald, A. M., Strack, J., & Coyne, J. C. (1981). Demand characteristics and the Velten mood induction procedure. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 49, 478-479.
Cash, T. F., Rimm, D. C., & MacKinnon, R. (1986). Rational–irrational beliefs and the effects of the Velten mood induction procedure. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 10, 461-467.
Clark, D. M. (1983). On the induction of depressed mood in the laboratory: Evaluation and comparison of the Velten and musical procedures. Advances in Behavioral Research and Therapy, 5, 27-49.
Cohen, J. (1977). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. New York: Academic Press.
Eich, E., Macaulay, D., & Ryan, L. (1994). Mood dependent memory for events of the personal past. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123, 201-215.
Ellenbogen, M. A., Young, S. N., Dean, P., Palmour, R. M., & Benekelfat, C. (1996). Mood response to acute tryptophan depletion in health volunteers: Sex differences and temporal stability. Neuropsychopharmacology, 15(5), 465-474.
First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. W. (1995). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders–-Patient Edition (SCID-I/P, Version 2.0). New York: New York State Psychiatric Institute, Biometrics Division.
Gerrards-Hesse, A., Kordelia, S., & Hesse, F. W. (1994). Experimental inductions of emotional states and their effectiveness: A review. British Journal of Psychology, 85, 55-78.
Gilboa-Schechtman, E., Revelle, W., & Gotlib, I.H. (2000). Stroop interference following mood induction: Emotionality, mood congruence and concern relevance. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 24, 491-502.
Gomez, R., Cooper, A., & Gomez, A. (2000). Susceptibility to positive and negative mood states: Test of Eysenck's, Gray's and Newman's theories. Personality and Individual Differences, 29, 351-366.
Ingram, R. E., & Ritter, J. (2000). Vulnerability to depression: Cognitive reactivity and parental bonding in high-risk individuals. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 588-596.
Kassel, J. D., & Unrod, M. (2000). Smoking, anxiety, and attention: Support for the role of nicotine in attentionally mediated anxiolysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109(1), 161-166.
Kenealy, P. M. (1986). The Velten mood induction procedure: A methodological review. Motivation and Emotion, 10, 315-335.
Larsen, R. J., & Sinnett, L. M. (1991). Meta-analysis of experimental manipulations: Some factors affecting the Velten mood induction procedure. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17, 323-334.
Martin, M. (1990). On the induction of mood. Clinical Psychology Review, 10, 669-697.
McNair, D. M., Lorr, M., & Droppleman, L. F. (1971). The Manual for the Profile of Mood States (POMS). San Diego, CA: Educational and Industrial Testing Services.
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Morrow, J. (1993). Effects of rumination and distraction on naturally occurring depressed mood. Cognition and Emotion 7(6), 561-570.
Norcross, J. C., Guadagnoli, E., & Prochaska, J. O. (1984). Factor structure of the Profile of Mood States (POMS): Two partial replications. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 40(5), 1270-1277.
Simes, R. J. (1986). An improved Bonferroni procedure for multiple tests of significance. Biometrica, 73(3), 751-754.
Slyker, J. P., & McNally, R. J. (1991). Experimental induction of anxious and depressed moods: Are Velten and musical procedures necessary? Cognitive Therapy and Research, 15(1), 33-45.
Spitzer, R. L., Williams, J. B., Gibbon, M., & First, M. B. (1994). The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID-IV). New York: New York State Psychiatric Institute, Biometrics Division.
Westermann, R., Spies, K., Stahl, G., Hesse, F. W. (1996). Relative effectiveness and validity of mood induction procedures: A meta-analysis. European Journal of Social Psychology, 26(4), 557-580.