Intermanual transfer of force control is modulated by asymmetry of muscular strength

Springer Science and Business Media LLC - Tập 149 - Trang 312-319 - 2003
Luis Augusto Teixeira1,2, Leandro Quedas Caminha1
1School of Physical Education and Sport, University of São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil
2Escola de Educação Física e Esporte, Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil

Tóm tắt

Interlateral transfer of learning is conceptualized as an index of the degree to which learning takes place at a lower level of motor control, with strong dependence on the effector system, or at a higher effector-independent level in the movement organization hierarchy. In this study, the locus of motor learning was investigated by increasing lateral asymmetry of force between the wrist flexor muscles, and comparing the amount of interlateral transfer of force control in relation to a condition of symmetric force. To perform this contrast, the participants were assigned to one of three groups: symmetric force (SM), who were left with original asymmetries of muscular strength; asymmetric force (AS), who had unilateral training for increment of maximum strength for the wrist flexor muscles; or a control condition (CO). The learning task consisted of launching a small cart across a metallic trackway with the preferred hand, aiming at making the cart achieve an instantaneous velocity of 70 cm/s. This action was practiced for 300 trials by the SM and AS group, while the CO group had active rest. The groups, then, were submitted to a transfer task requiring a mirrored action with the contralateral hand. The results indicated that the SM group achieved significantly higher interlateral transfer of learning as compared to the AS group, which presented response variability similar to the CO group. Analysis of directional trend of error revealed that the AS group presented a significant target overshoot as compared with the symmetric force groups. These findings suggest that an absolute force is learnt at a higher level in the action hierarchy, and that decline in interlateral transfer of learning in the asymmetric force condition was motivated by a resetting in the interplay between higher and lower levels of movement control.

Tài liệu tham khảo

Brinkman J, Kuypers HGJM (1973) Cerebral control of contralateral and ipsilateral arm, hand and finger movements in the split-brain rhesus monkey. Brain 96:653–674 Dizio P, Lackner JR (1995) Motor adaptation to coriolis force perturbations of reaching movements: endpoint but not trajectory adaptation transfers to the nonexposed arm. J Neurophysiol 74:1787–1792 Elbert T, Pantev C, Wienbruch C, Rockstroh B, Taub E (1995) Increased cortical representation of the fingers of the left hand in string players. Science 270:305–307 Gordon AM, Forssberg H, Iwasaki N (1994) Formation and lateralization of internal representations underlying motor commands during precision grip. Neuropsychologia 555–568 Hicks RE (1974) Asymmetry of bilateral transfer. Am J Psychol 87:667–674 Hollerbach JM (1990) Fundamentals of motor behavior. In: Osherson DN, Kosslyn SM, Hollerbach JM (eds) Visual cognition and action: an invitation to cognitive science (vol. 2). MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 153–182 Houston ME, Froese EA, Valeriote SP, Green HJ, Ranney DA (1983) Muscle performance, morphology and metabolic capacity during strength training and detraining: a one leg model. Eur J Appl Physiol 51:25–35 Imamizu H, Shimojo S (1995) The locus of visual-motor learning at the task or manipulator level: implications for intermanual transfer. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 21:719–733 Imamizu H, Uno Y, Kawato M (1998) Adaptive internal model of intrinsic kinematics involved in learning an aiming task. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 24:812–829 Ingram D (1975) Motor asymmetries in young children. Neuropsychologia 13:95–10 Ioffe M, Massion J, Gantchev N, Dufosse M, Kulikov MA (1996) Coordination between posture and movement in a bimanual load-lifting task: is there a transfer? Exp Brain Res 109:450–456 Karni A, Meyer G, Jezzard P, Adams MM, Turner R, Ungerleider LG (1995) Functional MRI evidence for adult motor cortex plasticity during skill learning. Nature 377:155–158 Kitazawa S, Kimura T, Uka T (1997) Prism adaptation of reaching movements: specificity for the velocity of reaching. J Neurosci 17:1481–1492 Komi PV, Viitasalo JT, Rauramaa R, Vihko V (1978) Effect of isometric strength training on mechanical, electrical, and metabolic aspects of muscle function. Eur J Appl Physiol 40:45–55 Laszlo JI, Baguley RA, Bairstow PJ (1970) Bilateral transfer in tapping skill in the absence of peripheral information. J Motor Behav 2:261–271 Lazarus J-AC, Haynes JM (1997) Isometric pinch force control and learning in older adults. Exp Aging Res 23:179–200 Moritani TMA, deVries HA (1979) Neural factors versus hypertrophy in the time course of muscle strength training. Am J Phys Med 58:115–130 Morton SM, Lang CE, Bastian AJ (2001) Inter- and intra-limb generalization of adaptation during catching. Exp Brain Res 141:438–445 Oldfield RC (1971) The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9:97–113 Park JH, Shea Ch (2002). Effector independence. J Motor Behav 34:253–270 Parlow SE, Kinsbourne M (1989) Asymmetrical transfer of training between hands: implications for interhemispheric communication in normal brain. Brain Cogn 11:98–113 Parlow SE, Kinsbourne M (1990) Asymmetrical transfer of Braille acquisition between hands. Brain Lang 39:319–330 Pascual-Leone A, Torres F (1993) Plasticity of the sensorimotor cortex representation of the reading finger in the Braille readers. Brain 116:39–52 Puretz SL (1983) Bilateral transfer: the effects of practice on the transfer of complex dance movement patterns. Res Q Exerc Sport 54:48–54 Recanzone GH, Merzenich MM, Jenkins WM, Grajski KA, Dinse HR (1992) Topographic reorganization of the hand representation in cortical area 3b of owl monkeys trained in a frequency-discrimination task. J Neurophysiol 67:1031–1056 Rigal R (1992) Which handedness: preference or performance? Percept Mot Skills 75:851–866 Salimi I, Hollender I, Frazier W, Gordon AM (2000) Specificity of internal representation underlying grasping. J Neurophysiol 84:2390–2397 Sathian K, Zangaladze A (1997) Tactile learning is task specific but transfers between fingers. Percept Psychophys 59:119–128 Sathian K, Zangaladze A (1998) Perceptual learning in tactile hyperacuity: complete intermanual transfer but limited retention. Exp Brain Res 118:131–134 Stoddard J, Vaid J (1996) Asymmetries in intermanual transfer of maze learning in right- and left-handed adults. Neuropsychologia 34:605–608 Taylor HG, Heilman KM (1980) Left-hemisphere motor dominance in right-handers. Cortex 16:587–603 Teixeira LA (1993) Bilateral transfer of learning: the effector side in focus. J Hum Mov Stud 25:243–253 Teixeira LA (1999) On what is transferred to one hand when grasping a moving ball is learnt with the other hand. Ciênc Cult 51:42–45 Teixeira LA (2000) Timing and force components in bilateral transfer of learning. Brain Cogn 44:455–469 Teixeira LA (2001) Variations in performance and lateral asymmetries with aging. In: Proceedings of the SCAPPS-2001 Annual Conference. Montreal, Canada, p 67 Teixeira LA, Paroli R (2000) Assimetrias laterais em ações motoras: preferência versus desempenho [lateral asymmetries in motor actions: preference versus performance]. Motriz 6:1–8 Thut G, Cook N, Regard M, Leenders K, Halsband U, Landis T (1996) Intermanual transfer of proximal and distal motor engrams in humans. Exp Brain Res 108:321–327 Weir JP, Housh TJ, Weir L (1994) Electromyographic evaluation of joint angle specificity and cross-training after isometric training. J Appl Physiol 77:197–201 Yasuda Y, Miyamura M (1983) Cross transfer effects of muscular training on blood flow in the ipsilateral and contralateral forearms. Eur J Appl Physiol 51:321–329 Yue G, Cole KJ (1992) Strength increases from the motor program: comparison of training with maximal voluntary and imagined muscle contractions. J Neurophysiol 67:1114–1123