6- to 10-year-old children do not show race-based orienting biases to faces during an online attention capture task
Tài liệu tham khảo
Amso, 2014, An eye tracking investigation of developmental change in bottom-up attention orienting to faces in cluttered natural scenes, PLoS One, 9, 10.1371/journal.pone.0085701
Anzures, 2014, Own- and other-race face identity recognition in children: The effects of pose and feature composition, Developmental Psychology, 50, 469, 10.1037/a0033166
Aslin, 2007, What’s in a look?, Developmental Science, 10, 48, 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00563.x
Belopolsky, 2007, The size of an attentional window modulates attentional capture by color singletons, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 934, 10.3758/BF03194124
Chiao, 2006, Priming race in biracial observers affects visual search for Black and White faces, Psychological Science, 17, 387, 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01717.x
Chun, 1996, Just say no: How are visual searches terminated when there is no target present?, Cognitive Psychology, 30, 39, 10.1006/cogp.1996.0002
Cohen, 1972, Attention-getting and attention-holding processes of infant visual preferences, Child Development, 43, 869, 10.2307/1127638
de Heering, 2010, The role of experience during childhood in shaping the other-race effect, Developmental Science, 13, 181, 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00876.x
Desimone, 1995, Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention, Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18, 193, 10.1146/annurev.ne.18.030195.001205
Di Giorgio, 2012, Is the face-perception system human-specific at birth?, Developmental Psychology, 48, 1083, 10.1037/a0026521
Duncan, 1989, Visual search and stimulus similarity, Psychological Review, 96, 433, 10.1037/0033-295X.96.3.433
Ellis, 2017, Scanning of own- versus other-race faces in infants from racially diverse or homogenous communities, Developmental Psychobiology, 59, 613, 10.1002/dev.21527
Erb, 2022, Attentional capture in goal-directed action during childhood, adolescence, and early adulthood, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 214, 10.1016/j.jecp.2021.105273
Estes, 1966, Reaction time in relation to display size and correctness of response in forced-choice visual signal detection, Perception & Psychophysics, 1, 369, 10.3758/BF03207411
Farroni, 2005, Newborns’ preference for face-relevant stimuli: Effects of contrast polarity, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102, 17245, 10.1073/pnas.0502205102
Fisher, 2019, Selective sustained attention: A developmental foundation for cognition, Current Opinion in Psychology, 29, 248, 10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.06.002
Gaspelin, 2014, Susceptible to distraction: Children lack top-down control over spatial attention capture, PsycExtra, 10.1037/e528942014-621
Gliga, 2009, Faces attract infants’ attention in complex displays, Infancy, 14, 550, 10.1080/15250000903144199
Godwin, 2014, The effects of increasing target prevalence on information processing during visual search, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22, 469, 10.3758/s13423-014-0686-2
Golarai, 2020, The development of race effects in face processing from childhood through adulthood: Neural and behavioral evidence, Developmental Science, 24, 10.1111/desc.13058
Goodman, 2007, The development of memory for own- and other-race faces, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 98, 233, 10.1016/j.jecp.2007.08.004
Hadders-Algra, 2022, Human face and gaze perception is highly context specific and involves bottom-up and top-down neural processing, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 132, 304, 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.11.042
Hunter, 1983, Effects of stimulus complexity and familiarization time on infant preferences for novel and familiar stimuli, Developmental Psychology, 19, 338, 10.1037/0012-1649.19.3.338
Hunter, 2021, Differential sensitivity to species- and race-based information in the development of attention orienting and attention holding face biases in infancy, Developmental Psychobiology, 63, 461, 10.1002/dev.22027
Hunter, 2022, Caregiver faces capture 6- to 10-year-old children’s attention during an online visual search task, Developmental Psychology
Jakobsen, 2016, Efficient human face detection in infancy, Developmental Psychobiology, 58, 129, 10.1002/dev.21338
Jeffreys, H. (1961). Theory of probability (3rd ed.). Oxford University Press.
Johnson, 2015, The two-process theory of face processing: Modifications based on two decades of data from infants and adults, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 50, 169, 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.10.009
Keebler, E., Kim, J., Stanley, O., Thiessen, E. D., & Fisher, A. V. (2020, July–August). Using the TrackIt task to measure the development of selective sustained attention in children ages 2–7. Paper presented at the 42nd annual meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (virtual). https://cognitivesciencesociety.org/cogsci20/papers/0552/0552.pdf.
Kelly, 2019, Infants rapidly detect human faces in complex naturalistic visual scenes, Developmental Science, 22, 10.1111/desc.12829
Kelly, 2009, Development of the other-race effect during infancy: Evidence toward universality?, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 104, 105, 10.1016/j.jecp.2009.01.006
Kelly, 2007, The other-race effect develops during infancy: Evidence of perceptual narrowing, Psychological Science, 18, 1084, 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.02029.x
Kelly, 2005, Three-month-olds, but not newborns, prefer own-race faces, Developmental Science, 8, F31, 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.0434a.x
Kwon, 2016, The development of visual search in infancy: Attention to faces versus salience, Developmental Psychology, 52, 537, 10.1037/dev0000080
Langton, 2008, Attention capture by faces, Cognition, 107, 330, 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.07.012
Leppänen, 2016, Using eye tracking to understand infants’ attentional bias for faces, Child Development Perspectives, 10, 161, 10.1111/cdep.12180
Levin, 1996, Classifying faces by race: The structure of face categories, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 1364
Liesefeld, 2021, Attentional capture: An ameliorable side-effect of searching for salient targets, Visual Cognition, 29, 600, 10.1080/13506285.2021.1925798
Liu, 2015, Development of visual preference for own- versus other-race faces in infancy, Developmental Psychology, 51, 500, 10.1037/a0038835
Lobaugh, 1998, Visual search for features and conjunctions in development, Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Expérimentale, 52, 201, 10.1037/h0087293
Ma, 2015, The Chicago Face Database: A free stimulus set of faces and norming data, Behavior Research Methods, 47, 1122, 10.3758/s13428-014-0532-5
Markant, 2018, Attention and perceptual learning interact in the development of the other-race effect, Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27, 163, 10.1177/0963721418769884
McKone, 2019, A critical period for faces: Other-race face recognition is improved by childhood but not adult social contact, Scientific Reports, 9, 10.1038/s41598-019-49202-0
Meissner, 2001, Thirty years of investigating the own-race bias in memory for faces: A meta-analytic review, Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 7, 3, 10.1037/1076-8971.7.1.3
Montoya, 2017, The effect of heterogeneous race exposure during infancy, Cognitive Development, 42, 74, 10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.02.014
Otten, 2016, Race guides attention in visual search, PLoS One, 11, 10.1371/journal.pone.0149158
Palermo, 2007, Are you always on my mind? A review of how face perception and attention interact, Neuropsychologia, 45, 75, 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.04.025
Palmer, 1994, Set-size effects in visual search: The effect of attention is independent of the stimulus for simple tasks, Vision Research, 34, 1703, 10.1016/0042-6989(94)90128-7
Peirce, 2019, PsychoPy2: Experiments in behavior made easy, Behavior Research Methods, 51, 195, 10.3758/s13428-018-01193-y
Peltier, 2016, Decision processes in visual search as a function of target prevalence, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42, 1466
Prunty, 2020, Caucasian infants’ attentional orienting to own- and other-race faces, Brain Sciences, 10, 10.3390/brainsci10010053
Riby, 2012, Faces cause less distraction in autism, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42, 634, 10.1007/s10803-011-1266-1
Sangrigoli, 2005, Reversibility of the other-race effect in face recognition during childhood, Psychological Science, 16, 440, 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01554.x
Singarajah, 2017, Infant attention to same- and other-race faces, Cognition, 159, 76, 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.11.006
Singh, 2022, Effects of interracial experience on the race preferences of infants, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 216, 10.1016/j.jecp.2021.105352
Theeuwes, 1998, Attentional control during visual search: The effect of irrelevant singletons, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1342
Treisman, 1988, Features and objects: The Fourteenth Bartlett Memorial Lecture, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A, 40, 201, 10.1080/02724988843000104
Trick, 1998, Lifespan changes in attention: The visual search task, Cognitive Development, 13, 369, 10.1016/S0885-2014(98)90016-8
Wolfe, 1998, What can 1 million trials tell us about visual search?, Psychological Science, 9, 33, 10.1111/1467-9280.00006
Wolfe, J. M. (2015). Visual search. In J. M. Fawcett, E. F. Risko, & A. Kingstone (Eds.), The handbook of attention (pp. 27–56). Boston Review.