Zooming in Time—Exploring Students’ Interpretations of a Dynamic Tree of Life

Springer Science and Business Media LLC - Tập 30 - Trang 125-138 - 2020
Jörgen Ingemar Stenlund1, Konrad Janek Schönborn2, Lena Anna Elisabet Tibell2
1School of Science and Technology, Örebro University, Örebro, Sweden
2Department of Science and Technology (ITN) Media and Information Technology (MIT), Linköping University, Linköping, Sweden

Tóm tắt

Central to evolution is the concept of a common ancestry from which all life has emerged over immense time scales, but learning and teaching temporal aspects of evolution remain challenging. This study investigated students’ interpretation of evolutionary time when engaging with a multi-touch tabletop application called DeepTree, a dynamic visualization of a phylogenetic tree. Specifically, we explored how interactive finger-based zooming (zooming “in” and “out”) influenced students’ interpretation of evolutionary time, and how temporal information and relationships were conceptualized during interaction. Transcript analysis of videotaped interview data from ten secondary school students while they interacted with DeepTree revealed that zooming was interpreted in two ways: as spatially orientated (movement within the tree itself), or as time-orientated (movement in time). Identified misinterpretations included perceiving an implicit coherent timeline along the y-axis of the tree, that the zooming time duration in the virtual tree was linearly correlated to real time, and that more branch nodes correspond to a longer time. Sources for erroneous interpretations may lie in transferring everyday sensory experiences (e.g., physical movements and observing tree growth) to understanding abstract evolution concepts. Apart from estimating the occurrence of dinosaurs, DeepTree was associated with an improvement in interpretation of relative order of evolutionary events. Although highly promising, zooming interaction in DeepTree does not facilitate an intuitive understanding of evolutionary time. However, the opportunity to combine visual and bodily action in emerging technologies such as Deep Tree suggests a high pedagogical potential of further development of zooming features for optimal scientific understanding.

Tài liệu tham khảo

Ainsworth, S. (2006). DeFT: A conceptual framework for considering learning with multiple representations. Learning and Instruction, 16(3), 183–198. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2006.03.001. Ainsworth, S., & VanLabeke, N. (2004). Multiple forms of dynamic representation. Learning and Instruction, 14(3), 241–255. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2004.06.002. Amundsen, C., Weston, C., & McAlpine, L. (2008). Concept mapping to support university academics’ analysis of course content. Studies in Higher Education, 33(6), 633–652. https://doi.org/10.1080/03075070802373180. Athanasiou, K., & Mavrikaki, E. (2014). Conceptual inventory of natural selection as a tool for measuring Greek university students’ evolution knowledge: Differences between novice and advanced students. International Journal of Science Education, 36(8), 1262–1285. https://doi.org/10.1080/09500693.2013.856529. Baum, D. A., Smith, S. D., & Donovan, S. S. (2005). The tree-thinking challenge. Science, 310(5750), 979. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1117727. Bederson, B. B. (2011). The promise of zoomable user interfaces. Behaviour & Information Technology, 30(6), 451 853–866. https://doi.org/10.1080/0144929X.2011.586724. Bell, P. (2001). Content analysis of visual images. Handbook of visual analysis, 13. Block, F., Horn, M. S., Phillips, B. C., Diamond, J., Evans, E. M., & Shen, C. (2012a). The deeptree exhibit: Visualizing the tree of life to facilitate informal learning. IEEE Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics, 18(12), 2789–2798. Block, F., Wigdor, D., Phillips, B. C., Horn, M. S., & Shen, C. (2012b). FlowBlocks: A multi-touch ui for crowd interaction. In Proceedings of the 25th annual ACM symposium on User interface software and technology (pp. 497–508). https://doi.org/10.1145/2380116.2380178. Boroditsky, L. (2000). Metaphoric structuring: Understanding time through spatial metaphors. Cognition, 75(1), 1–28. Bos, W., & Tarnai, C. (1999). Content analysis in empirical social research. International Journal of Educational Research, 31(8), 659–671. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0883-0355(99)00032-4. Börner, K., Chen, C., & Boyack, K. W. (2005). Visualizing knowledge domains. Annual Review of Information Science and Technology, 37(1), 179–255. https://doi.org/10.1002/aris.1440370106. Catley, K. M., & Novick, L. R. (2009). Digging deep: Exploring college students’ knowledge of macroevolutionary time. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 46(3), 311–332. https://doi.org/10.1002/tea.20273. Catley, K. M., Phillips, B. C., & Novick, L. R. (2013). Snakes and eels and dogs! Oh, my! Evaluating high school students’ tree-thinking skills: An entry point to understanding evolution. Research in Science Education, 43(6), 2327–2348. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11165-013-9359-9. Cheek, K. A. (2013a). Exploring the relationship between students’ understanding of conventional time and deep (geologic) time. International Journal of Science Education, 35(11), 1925–1945. https://doi.org/10.1080/09500693.2011.587032. Cheek, K. A. (2013b). How geoscience novices reason about temporal duration: The role of spatial thinking and large numbers. Journal of Geoscience Education, 61(3), 334–348. Darwin, C. (1859). On the origin of species by means of natural selection. London: Murray. Davis, P., Horn, M., Block, F., Phillips, B., Evans, E. M., Diamond, J., & Shen, C. (2015). “Whoa! We’re going deep in the trees!”: Patterns of collaboration around an interactive information visualization exhibit. International Journal of Computer-Supported Collaborative Learning, 10(1), 53–76. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11412-015-9209-z. Delgado, C., & Lucero, M. M. (2015). Scale construction for graphing: An investigation of students’ resources. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 52(5), 633–658. https://doi.org/10.1002/tea.21205. Dodick, J., & Orion, N. (2003). Cognitive factors affecting student understanding of geologic time. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 40(4), 415–442. https://doi.org/10.1002/tea.10083. Fereday, J., & Muir-Cochrane, E. (2006). Demonstrating rigor using thematic analysis: A hybrid approach of inductive and deductive coding and theme development. International Journal of Qualitative Methods, 5(1), 80–92. https://doi.org/10.1177/160940690600500107. Foreman, N. (2008). Can virtual environments enhance the learning of historical chronology? Instructional Science, 36(2), 155–173. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-007-9024-7. Gregory, T. R. (2008). Understanding evolutionary trees. Evolution: Education and Outreach, 1(2), 121–137. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12052-008-0035-x18. Hecht, M., Knutson, K., Crowley, K., Lyon, M., McShea, P., & Giarratani, L. (2020). ‘How could the dinosaurs be so close to the future?’: How natural history museum educators tackle deep time. Curator: The Museum Journal, 63(1), 39–54. https://doi.org/10.1111/cura.12342. Hidalgo, A. J., Fernando, I. E. S. S., & Otero, I. C. E. J. (2004). Research Report: An analysis 502 of the understanding of geological time by students at secondary and post- secondary level. International Journal of Science Education, 26(7), 845–857. https://doi.org/10.1080/0950069032000119438. Horn, M. S., Phillips, B. C., Evans, E. M., Block, F., Diamond, J., & Shen, C. (2016). Visualizing biological data in museums: Visitor learning with an interactive tree of life exhibit. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 53(6), 895–918. https://doi.org/10.1002/tea.21318. Hornecker, E., Marshall, P., Dalton, N. S., & Rogers, Y. (2008). Collaboration and interference: Awareness with mice or touch input. Proceedings of the ACM 2008 Conference on Computer Supported Cooperative Work - CSCW ’08, 167. https://doi.org/10.1145/1460563.1460589. Höst, G. E., & Anward, J. (2017). Intentions and actions in molecular self-assembly: Perspectives on students’ language use. International Journal of Science Education, 39(6), 627–644. https://doi.org/10.1080/09500693.2017.1298870. Jaimes, P., Libarkin, J. C., & Conrad, D. (2020). College student conceptions about changes to earth and life over time. CBE—Life Sciences Education, 19(3), ar35. https://doi.org/10.1187/cbe.19-01-0008. Johnson-Glenberg, M. C., Birchfield, D. A., Tolentino, L., & Koziupa, T. (2014). Collaborative embodied learning in mixed reality motion-capture environments: Two science studies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 106(1), 86–104. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034008. Korallo, L., Foreman, N., Boyd-Davis, S., Moar, M., & Coulson, M. (2012). Can multiple “spatial” virtual timelines convey the relatedness of chronological knowledge across parallel domains? Computers & Education, 58(2), 856–862. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2011.10.011. Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the flesh: The embodied mind and its challenge to Western thought. New York: New York : Basic Books. Lee, C., Devillers, R., & Hoeber, O. (2014). Navigating spatio-temporal data with temporal zoom and pan in a multi-touch environment. International Journal of Geographical Information Science, 28(5), 1128–1148. https://doi.org/10.1080/13658816.2013.861072. Lee, H.-S., Liu, O. L., Price, C. A., & Kendall, A. L. M. (2011). College students’ temporal-magnitude recognition ability associated with durations of scientific changes. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 48(3), 317–335. https://doi.org/10.1002/tea.204012007. Libarkin, J. C., Kurdziel, J. P., & Anderson, S. W. (2007). College student conceptions of geological time and the disconnect between ordering and scale. Journal of Geoscience Education, 55(5), 413–422. Marcelos, M. F., & Nagem, R. L. (2010). Comparative structural models of similarities and differences between vehicle and target in order to teach darwinian evolution. Science & Education, 19(6–8), 599–623. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11191-009-9218-2. Matuk, C., & Uttal, D. H. (2018). The effects of invention and recontextualization on representing and reasoning with trees of life. Research in Science Education. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11165-018-9761-4. Mayer, R. E., & Moreno, R. (2003). Nine ways to reduce cognitive load in multimedia learning. Educational Psychologist, 38(1), 43–52. Mayring, P. (2000). Qualitative content analysis. Qualitative Content Analysis. Forum: Qualitative Social Research, 1(2). Meir, E., Perry, J., Herron, J. C., & Kingsolver, J. (2007). College students’ misconceptions about evolutionary trees. The American Biology Teacher, 69(7), 71–76. https://doi.org/10.1662/0002-7685(2007)69[71:CSMAET]2.0.CO;2. Moreno, R., & Mayer, R. (2007). Interactive multimodal learning environments: Special issue on interactive learning environments: Contemporary issues and trends. Educational Psychology Review, 19(3), 309–326. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-007-9047-2. Nemirovsky, R., & Tierney, C. (2001). Children creating ways to represent changing situations: On the development of homogeneous spaces. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 45(1/3), 67–102. O’Malley, M. A., & Koonin, E. V. (2011). How stands the Tree of Life a century and a half after The Origin? Biology Direct, 6(1), 32. https://doi.org/10.1186/1745-6150-6-32. Omland, K. E., Cook, L. G., & Crisp, M. D. (2008). Tree thinking for all biology: The problem with reading phylogenies as ladders of progress. BioEssays, 30(9), 854–867. https://doi.org/10.1002/bies.20794. Phillips, L. M., Norris, S. P., & Macnab, J. S. (2010). Visualization in mathematics, reading and science education. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1007/978-90-481-8816-1. Schönborn, K. J., & Anderson, T. R. (2009). A Model of Factors Determining Students’ Ability to Interpret External Representations in Biochemistry. International Journal of Science Education, 31(2), 193–232. https://doi.org/10.1080/09500690701670535. Schuman, C., Stofer, K. A., Anthony, L., Neff, H., Chang, P., Soni, N., et al. (2020). Ocean data visualization on a touchtable demonstrates group content learning, science practices use, and potential embodied cognition. Research in Science Education. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11165-020-09951-9. Smith, M. U. (2010). Current status of research in teaching and learning evolution: II. Pedagogical issues. Science & Education, 19(6–8), 539–571. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11191-009-9216-4. Stenlund, J. I., & Tibell, L. A. E. (2019). Visualizing macroevolutionary timescales: Students’ comprehension of different temporal representations in an animation. Evolution: Education and Outreach, 12(1). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12052-019-0099-9 Swarat, S., Light, G., Park, E. J., & Drane, D. (2011). A typology of undergraduate students’ conceptions of size and scale: Identifying and characterizing conceptual variation. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 48(5), 512–533. https://doi.org/10.1002/tea.20403. Tang, K.-S., Delgado, C., & Moje, E. B. (2014). An integrative framework for the analysis of multiple and multimodal representations for meaning-making in science education. Science Education, 98(2), 305–326. https://doi.org/10.1002/sce.21099. Trend, R. (1998). An investigation into understanding of geological time among 10- and 11- year- old children. International Journal of Science Education, 20(8), 973–988. https://doi.org/10.1080/0950069980200805. Trend, R. (2000). Conceptions of geological time among primary teacher trainees, with reference to their engagement with geoscience, history, and science. International Journal of Science Education, 22(5), 539–555. https://doi.org/10.1080/095006900289778. Trend, R. (2001). An investigation into the understanding of geological time among 17-year-old students, with implications for the subject matter knowledge of future teachers. International Research in Geographical and Environmental Education, 10(3), 298–321. https://doi.org/10.1080/10382040108667447.