Women Who Know Their Place
Tóm tắt
Differences between men and women in the performance of tests designed to measure spatial abilities are explained by evolutionary psychologists in terms of adaptive design. The Hunter-Gatherer Theory of Spatial Ability suggests that the adoption of a hunter-gatherer lifestyle (assuming a sexual division of labor) created differential selective pressure on the development of spatial skills in men and women and, therefore, cognitive differences between the sexes. Here, we examine a basic spatial skill—wayfinding (the ability to plan routes and navigate a landscape)—in men and women in a natural, real-world setting as a means of testing the proposition that sex-based differences in spatial ability exist outside of the laboratory. Our results indicate that when physical differences are accounted for, men and women with equivalent experience perform equally well at complex navigation tasks in a real-world setting. We conclude that experience, gendered patterns of activity, and self-assessment are contributing factors in producing previously reported differences in spatial ability.
Tài liệu tham khảo
Baenninger, M., & Newcombe, N. (1989). The role of experience in spatial test performance: a meta-analysis. Sex Roles, 20, 327–344.
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: the exercise of control. New York: W.H. Freeman.
Barnfield, A. M. C. (1999). Development of sex differences in spatial memory. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 89(1), 339–350.
Beatty, W. W. (2002). Sex difference in geographical knowledge: driving experience is not essential. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8(6), 804–810.
Bosco, A., Longoni, A. M., & Vecchi, T. (2004). Gender effects in spatial orientation: cognitive profiles and mental strategies. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18(5), 519–532.
Burgess, N. (2006). Spatial memory: how egocentric and allocentric combine. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(12), 551–557.
Choi, J., McKillop, E., Ward, M., & L’Hirondelle, N. (2006). Sex-specific relationships between route-learning strategies and abilities in a large-scale environment. Environment and Behavior, 38(6), 791–801.
Coluccia, E., & Louse, G. (2004). Gender differences in spatial orientation: a review. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 24(3), 329–340.
Dabbs, J. M., Chang, E. L., Strong, R. A., & Milun, R. (1998). Spatial ability, navigation strategy, and geographic knowledge among men and women. Evolution and Human Behavior, 19(2), 89–98.
Eals, M., & Silverman, I. (1994). The hunter-gatherer theory of spatial sex differences: proximate factors mediating the female advantage in recall of object arrays. Ethology and Sociobiology, 15(2), 95–105.
Feng, J., Spence, I., & Pratt, J. (2007). Playing an action video game reduces gender differences in spatial cognition. Psychological Science, 18, 850–855.
Foo, P., Warren, W. H., Duchon, A., & Tarr, M. J. (2005). Do humans integrate routes into a cognitive map? Map- versus landmark-based navigation of novel shortcuts. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31(2), 195–215.
Gibson, E. J., & Bergman, R. (1954). The effect of training on absolute estimation of distance over the ground. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48(6), 473–482.
Gilmartin, P. P., & Patton, J. C. (1984). Comparing the sexes on spatial abilities: map-use skills. Annals of the Association of American Geographers, 74(4), 605–619. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8306.1984.tb01477.x.
Golledge, R. G. (1999). Human wayfinding and cognitive maps. In R. G. Golledge (Ed.), Wayfinding behaviour (pp. 5–45). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.
Golledge, R. G. (2003). Human wayfinding and cognitive maps. In M. Rockman & J. Steele (Eds.), Colonization of unfamiliar landscapes: the archaeology of adaptation (pp. 25–43). London and New York: Routledge.
Golledge, R. G., Ruggles, A. J., Pellegrino, J. W., & Gale, N. D. (1993). Integrating route knowledge in an unfamiliar neighborhood: along and across route experiments. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 13(4), 293–307.
Hegarty, M., Montello, D. R., Richardson, A. E., Ishikawa, T., & Lovelace, K. (2006). Spatial abilities at different scales: individual differences in aptitude-test performance and spatial-layout learning. Intelligence, 34(2), 151–176.
Istomin, K. V., & Dwyer, M. J. (2009). Finding the way: a critical discussion of anthropological theories of human spatial orientation with reference to reindeer herders of Northeastern Europe and Western Siberia. Current Anthropology, 50(1), 29–49.
James, T. W., & Kimura, D. (1997). Sex differences in remembering the locations of objects in an array: location-shifts versus location-exchanges. Evolution and Human Behavior, 18(3), 155–163.
Janzen, G., Jansen, C., & van Turennout, M. (2007). Memory consolidation of landmarks in good navigators. Hippocampus, 18(1), 40–47.
Johansen, B. T. (1997). Thinking in orienteering. Scientific Journal of Orienteering, 13, 38–46.
Jones, C. M., Braithwaite, V. A., & Healy, S. D. (2003). The evolution of sex differences in spatial ability. Behavioral Neuroscience, 117(3), 403–411.
Kitchin, R. M. (1994). Cognitive maps: what are they and why study them? Journal of Environmental Psychology, 14(1), 1–19.
Lawton, C. A. (1994). Gender differences in way-finding strategies: relationship to spatial ability and spatial anxiety. Sex Roles, 30(11–12), 765–779.
Lawton, C. A. (1996). Strategies for indoor wayfinding: the role of orientation. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 16(2), 137–145.
Lawton, C. A. (2001). Gender and regional differences in spatial referents used in direction giving. Sex Roles, 44(5–6), 321–337.
Levy, L. J., Astur, R. S., & Frick, K. M. (2005). Men and women differ in object memory but not performance of a virtual radial maze. Behavioral Neuroscience, 119(4), 853–862.
Linn, M. C., & Petersen, A. C. (1985). Emergence and characterization of sex differences in spatial ability: a meta-analysis. Child Development, 56(6), 1479–1498.
Lobben, A. K. (2004). Tasks, strategies, and cognitive processes associated with navigational map reading: a review perspective. The Professional Geographer, 56(2), 270–281.
Loomis, J. M., Klatzky, R. L., Golledge, R. G., & Philbeck, J. W. (1999). Human navigation by path integration. In R. G. Golledge (Ed.), Wayfinding behavior (pp. 125–151). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.
Maguire, A. M., Spiers, H. J., Good, C. D., Hartley, T., Frackowiak, R. S. J., & Burgess, N. (2003). Navigation expertise and the human hippocampus: a structural brain imaging analysis. Hippocampus, 13(2), 250–259.
Malinowski, J. C. (2001). Mental rotation and real-world wayfinding. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 92(1), 19–30.
Malinowski, J. C., & Gillespie, W. T. (2001). Individual differences in performance on a large-scale, real-world wayfinding task. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 21(1), 73–82.
Mau, W. C., & Bikos, L. H. (2000). Educational and vocational aspirations of minority and female students: a longitudinal study. Journal of Counseling and Development, 78(2), 186–194.
McBurney, D. H., Gaulin, S. J. C., Devineni, T., & Adams, C. (1997). Superior spatial memory of women: stronger evidence for the gathering hypothesis. Evolution and Human Behavior, 18(3), 165–174.
Montello, D. R., Lovelace, K. L., Golledge, R. G., & Self, C. M. (1999). Sex-related differences and similarities in geographic and environmental spatial abilities. Annals of the Association of American Geographers, 89(3), 515–534.
Myrvold, B. (1996). Is it possible to find a “best” route? A look at accuracy and significance in route choice comparison. Scientific Journal of Orienteering, 12, 19–36.
Nadolne, M. J., & Stringer, A. Y. (2001). Ecologic validity in neuropsychological assessment: prediction of wayfinding. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 7(6), 675–682.
Neave, N., Hamilton, C., Hutton, L., Tildesley, N., & Pickering, A. T. (2005). Some evidence of a female advantage in object location memory using ecologically valid stimuli. Human Nature, 16(2), 146–163.
New, J., Krasnow, M. M., Truxaw, D., & Gaulin, S. J. C. (2007). Spatial adaptations for plant foraging: women excel and calories count. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences, 274(1626), 2679–2684.
O’Brien, V., Martinez-Pons, M., & Kopala, M. (1999). Mathematics self-efficacy, ethnic identity, gender, and career interests related to mathematics and science. The Journal of Educational Research, 92(4), 231–235.
Pajares, F., & Miller, M. D. (1994). Role of self-efficacy and self-concept beliefs in mathematical problem-solving: a path-analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86(2), 193–203.
Postma, A., Jager, G., Kessels, R. P. C., Koppeschaar, H. P. F., & van Honk, J. (2004). Sex differences for selective forms of spatial memory. Brain and Cognition, 54(1), 24–34.
Quaiser-Pohl, C., & Lehmann, W. (2002). Girls’ spatial abilities: charting the contributions of experiences and attitudes in different academic groups. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 72, 245–260.
Randhawa, B. S. (1994). Self-efficacy in mathematics, attitudes, and achievements of boys and girls from restricted samples in 2 countries. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 79(2), 1011–1018.
Reis, S. M., & Park, S. (2001). Gender differences in high-achieving students in math and science. Journal for the Education of the Gifted, 25(1), 52–73.
Rilea, S. L., Roskos-Ewoldsen, B., & Boles, D. (2004). Sex differences in spatial ability: a lateralization of function approach. Brain and Cognition, 56(3), 332–343.
Sandstrom, N. J., Kaufman, J., & Huettel, S. A. (1998). Males and females use different distal cues in a virtual environment navigation task. Cognitive Brain Research, 6(4), 351–360.
Saucier, D. M., McCreary, D. R., & Saxberg, J. K. J. (2002). Does gender role socialization mediate sex differences in mental rotations? Personality and Individual Differences, 32(6), 1101–1111.
Shelton, A. L., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2004). Neural correlates of individual differences in spatial learning strategies. Neuropsychology, 18(3), 442–449.
Silverman, I., Choi, J., Mackewn, A., Fisher, M., Moro, J., & Olshansky, E. (2000). Evolved mechanisms underlying wayfinding: further studies on the hunter-gatherer theory of spatial sex differences. Evolution and Human Behavior, 21(3), 201–213.
Silverman, I., and Eals, M. (1992). Sex differences in spatial abilities: evolutionnary theory and data. In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides and L. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind: evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 487–503). Oxford University Press.
Tottenham, L. S., Saucier, D., Elias, L., & Gutwin, C. (2003). Female advantage for spatial location memory in both static and dynamic environments. Brain and Cognition, 53(2), 381–383.
Voyer, D., Postma, A., Brake, B., & Imperato-McGinley, J. (2007). Gender differences in object location memory: a meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 14(1), 23–38.
Voyer, D., Voyer, S., & Bryden, M. P. (1995). Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities: a meta-analysis and consideration of critical variables. Psychological Bulletin, 117(2), 250–270.
Whitaker, L. A., & Cuqlock-Knopp, G. (1992). Navigation in off-road environments: orienteering interviews. Scientific Journal of Orienteering, 8, 55–71.
Wynn, T. G., Tierson, F. D., & Palmer, C. T. (1996). Evolution of sex differences in spatial cognition. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology, 39, 11–42.