Why People “Don’t Trust the Evidence”

Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science - Tập 658 Số 1 - Trang 121-133 - 2015
Patrick Kraft, Milton Lodge, Charles S. Taber

Tóm tắt

In this commentary, we embed the volume’s contributions on public beliefs about science in a broader theoretical discussion of motivated political reasoning. The studies presented in the preceding section of the volume consistently find evidence for hyperskepticism toward scientific evidence among ideologues, no matter the domain or context—and this skepticism seems to be stronger among conservatives than liberals. Here, we show that these patterns can be understood as part of a general tendency among individuals to defend their prior attitudes and actively challenge attitudinally incongruent arguments, a tendency that appears to be evident among liberals and conservatives alike. We integrate the empirical results reported in this volume into a broader theoretical discussion of the John Q. Public model of information processing and motivated reasoning, which posits that both affective and cognitive reactions to events are triggered unconsciously. We find that the work in this volume is largely consistent with our theories of affect-driven motivated reasoning and biased attitude formation.

Từ khóa


Tài liệu tham khảo

Ajzen Icek, 1980, Understanding attitudes and predicting social behavior

10.1038/nn1979

Bargh John A., 1999, Dual-process theories in social psychology, 361

10.1093/poq/nft044

10.1007/s11109-013-9238-0

10.1371/journal.pone.0026456

10.1146/annurev.polisci.10.072805.103054

10.1017/S0022381613000492

Converse Philip E., 1964, Ideology and discontent, 206

10.1126/science.1188595

Damasio Antonio, 1994, Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason and the human brain

10.1111/pops.12075

10.1037/0022-3514.56.1.5

10.1111/j.1460-2466.2011.01562.x

10.1177/0146167212439213

10.1521/soco.2007.25.5.687

10.1017/S0003055410000031

10.1037/a0015141

10.1037/0033-295X.102.1.4

10.1037/0033-2909.129.3.339

10.1017/S1930297500005271

Kruglanski Arie W., 2004, Psychology of closed-mindedness

10.1080/08913811.2012.711023

10.1037/0033-2909.108.3.480

10.1017/S0140525X00044903

10.1017/CBO9781139032490

10.1037/0022-3514.37.11.2098

10.1177/0146167298247004

10.1037/0033-2909.120.3.323

10.1111/j.1533-8525.2011.01198.x

10.1037/h0043158

10.1023/A:1024834831093

10.1037/0033-295X.106.2.337

Olson Michael A., 2009, Attitudes: Insights from the New Implicit Measures, 86, 19

10.1002/per.775

10.1111/j.1467-9221.2010.00772.x

10.1111/j.1467-9221.2010.00764.x

10.1007/s11109-008-9075-8

10.1111/j.1540-5907.2006.00214.x

10.1080/08913811.2012.719664

Talhelm Thomas, Haidt Jonathan, Oishi Shigehiro, Zhang Xuemin, Miao Felicity, Chen Shimin. 2012. Liberals think more analytically (more “weird”) than conservatives. Social Science Research Network Working Paper. Available from http://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=2111700.

Thagard Paul, 1998, Connectionist models of social reasoning and social behavior, 3

Vertzberger Yaacov, 1990, The world in their minds: Information processing, cognition, and perception in foreign policy decision-making

10.1037/0033-295X.107.1.101

Young Everett H. 2009. Why we’re liberal, why we’re conservative: A cognitive theory on the origins of ideological thinking. PhD thesis, State University of New York at Stony Brook.

10.1037/0003-066X.35.2.151

10.1017/CBO9780511818691

10.2307/2111583