What do architects and students perceive in their design sketches? A protocol analysis

Design Studies - Tập 18 Số 4 - Trang 385-403 - 1997
Masaki Suwa1,2, Barbara Tversky1,2
1Advanced Research Laboratory, Hitachi Ltd. 2520, Hatoyama, Saitama 350-03, Japan
2Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305 USA

Tóm tắt

Từ khóa


Tài liệu tham khảo

Gelernter, 1963, Realization of a geometry-theorem proving machine

McDougal, 1992, A recognition model of geometry theorem-proving, 106

Koedinger, 1990, Abstract planning and perceptual chunks: elements of expertise in geometry, Cognitive Science, Vol 14, 511, 10.1207/s15516709cog1404_2

Suwa, 1994, PCLEARN: a model for learning perceptual-chunks, 830

Petre M‘Why looking isn't always seeing: readership skills and graphical programming’ Communications of the ACM, Vol 38 No 6 33–44

Larkin, 1987, Why a diagram is (sometimes) worth ten thousand words, Cognitive Science, Vol 11, 65, 10.1111/j.1551-6708.1987.tb00863.x

Tversky, 1995, Cognitive origins of graphic conventions, 29

Tversky, 1995, Perception and cognition of 2D and 3D graphics, 175

Miyake, 1991, Socio-cultural constraints and beyond, Progresses of Cognitive Science (in Japanese), Vol 2, 105

Laseau, 1989

Robbins, 1994

Schon, 1992, Kinds of seeing and their functions in designing, Design Studies, Vol 13, 135, 10.1016/0142-694X(92)90268-F

Goldschmidt, 1991, The dialectics of sketching, Creativity Research Journal, Vol 4, 123, 10.1080/10400419109534381

Goldschmidt, 1994, On visual design thinking: the via kids of architecture, Design Studies, Vol 15, 158, 10.1016/0142-694X(94)90022-1

Barwise, 1990, Visual information and valid reasoning

Stenning, 1995, A cognitive theory of graphical and linguistic reasoning: logic and implementation, Cognitive Science, Vol 19, 97, 10.1207/s15516709cog1901_3

Goel, 1995

Goodman, 1968

Van Sommers, 1984

Ericsson, 1993

Dorst, 1995, Pro-tocol analysis as a research technique for analysing design activity, Vol 2, 563

Lloyd, 1995, Can concurrent verbalization reveal design cognition, Design Studies, Vol 16, 237, 10.1016/0142-694X(94)00011-2

Landay, 1995, Interactive sketching for the early stages of user interface design, 43

Kramer, 1994, Translucent patches — dissolving windows, 121

Gross, 1996, The electronic Cocktail Napkin — a computational environment for working with design diagrams, Design Studies, Vol 17, 53, 10.1016/0142-694X(95)00006-D

Liu, 1995, Some phenomena of seeing shapes in design, Design Studies, Vol 16, 367, 10.1016/0142-694X(94)00001-T

Arnheim, 1977

Eastman, 1970, On the analysis of intuitive design processes, 21

Krauss, 1970, Design: a case history, 11

Akin, 1986

Akin, 1993, ‘Architects’ reasoning with structures and functions, Environment and Planning B: Planning and Design, Vol 20, 273, 10.1068/b200273

Chan, 1990, Cognitive processes in architectural design problem solving, Design Studies, Vol 11, 60, 10.1016/0142-694X(90)90021-4

Dorst, 1995, Comparing paradigms for describing design activity, Design Studies, Vol 16, 261, 10.1016/0142-694X(94)00012-3

Schon, 1983

Miller, 1956, The magical number seven plus or minus two: some limits on your capacity for processing information, Psychological Review, Vol 63, 81, 10.1037/h0043158

Chase, 1973, Perception in chess, Cognitive psychology, Vol 4, 55, 10.1016/0010-0285(73)90004-2

Goldschmidt, 1992, Criteria for design evaluation: a process-oriented paradigm

Suwa M and Gross M D ‘Enriching perception: a new paradigm for design sketching tools’ (personal manuscript)

Suwa, 1996, What architects see in their design sketches: implications for design tools, 191