Toward a theory of the perceived spatial layout of scenes
Tài liệu tham khảo
Arbie, 1973, Multidimensional scaling of measures of distance between partitions, J. Math. Psychol., 10, 148, 10.1016/0022-2496(73)90012-6
Attneave, 1950, Dimensions of similarity, Amer. J. Psychol., 63, 516, 10.2307/1418869
Barrow, 1978, Recovering intrinsic scene characteristics from imagex
Baum, 1979, Cognitive maps: Analysis of comparative judgments of distance, Mem. and Cognit., 7, 462, 10.3758/BF03198262
Bengston, 1980, Optic array determinants of apparent distance and size in pictures, J. Exp. Psychol., Human Percept. and Perform., 6, 751, 10.1037/0096-1523.6.4.751
Biederman, 1981, On the semantics of a glance at a scene, 213
Biederman, 1981, Do background depth gradients facilitate object identification?, Perception, 10, 573, 10.1068/p100573
Brabyn, 1977, Computer-analysed measures of characteristics of human locomotion and mobility, Behav. Res. Methods and Instrum., 9, 456, 10.3758/BF03202286
Bradley, 1984, Psychophysical functions for perceived and remembered distance, Perception, 13, 315, 10.1068/p130315
Brady, 1982
Brady, 1982, Artif. Intell., 18
Braunstein, 1981, Velocity gradients and relative depth perception, Percept. & Psychophys., 29, 145, 10.3758/BF03207278
Brown, 1937, The path of seen movement as a function of the vector-field, American J. Psychol., 44, 543, 10.2307/1416380
Byrne, 1983, Distances and directions in the cognitive map of the blind, Can. J. Psychol., 37, 293, 10.1037/h0080726
Campbell, 1971, The tilt after-effect: A fresh look, Vision Res., 11, 833, 10.1016/0042-6989(71)90005-8
J. D. Carrol and J. J. Chang, Analysis of individual differences in multi-dimensional scaling via an n-way generalization of “Eckart-Young” decomposition, Psychometrika 35,238–319.
J. A. Da Silva and A. Dos Santos, The effects of instructions on scales for perceived egocentric distance in a large open field, Bull. Psychon. Sot. 22,189–192.
Davidson, 1975
Degelman, 1979, Motion parallax and children's distance perception, Dev. Psychol., 15, 147, 10.1037/0012-1649.15.2.147
De Renzi, 1982
Dillon, 1983
Dolezal, 1982
Epstein, 1977
Epstein, 1964, Examination of Gibson's psychophysical hypothesis, Psychol. Bull., 62, 180, 10.1037/h0044097
Eriksson, 1974, Movement parallax during locomotion, Percept. and Psychophys., 16, 197, 10.3758/BF03203274
Farber, 1979, Optical motions as information for unsigned depth, J. Exper. Psychol., Human Percept. and Perform., 5, 494, 10.1037/0096-1523.5.3.494
Feldman, 1985, Four frames suffice: A provisional model of vision and space, Behav. and Brain Sci., 8, 10.1017/S0140525X00020707
Ferris, 1972, Motion parallax and absolute distance, J. Exp. Psychol., 95, 258, 10.1037/h0033605
Fitch, 1978, On the control of activity: Some remarks from an ecological point of view
Foley, 1984, Mental mapping of a megastructure, Can. J. Psychol., 38, 440, 10.1037/h0080853
Foley, 1980, Binocular distance perception, Psychol. Rev., 87, 411, 10.1037/0033-295X.87.5.411
Foley, 1972, visually directed pointing as a function of target distance, direction and available cues, Percept. and Psychophys., 12, 263, 10.3758/BF03207201
Gibson, 1954, Motion parallax as a determinant of perceived depth, J. Exp. Psychol., 58, 40, 10.1037/h0043883
Gibson, 1950
Gibson, 1977, On the analysis of change in the optic array, Scand. J. Psychol., 18, 161, 10.1111/j.1467-9450.1977.tb00272.x
Gibson, 1979
Gibson, 1952, Does motion perspective independently produce the impressing of a receding surface?, J. Exp. Psychol., 44, 16, 10.1037/h0056030
Gibson, 1955, A method for controlling stimulation for the study of space perception: The optical tunnel, J. Exp. Psych., 50, 1, 10.1037/h0043072
Gilinsky, 1951, Perceived size and distance in visual space, Psychol. Rev., 58, 460, 10.1037/h0061505
Gogel, 1963, The Visual Perception of Spatial Extent, FAA-CARI document No. 63-20, Oklahoma City
Gogel, 1974, cognitive factors in spatial responses, Psychologica, 17, 213
Gogel, 1976, An indirect method of measuring perceived distance from familiar size, Percept. and Psychophys., 20, 419, 10.3758/BF03208276
Gogel, 1979, Depth adjacency and induced motion, Percept. and Motor Skills, 48, 343, 10.2466/pms.1979.48.2.343
Gogel, 1968, Perceived depth between familiar objects, J. Exp. Psychol., 77, 206, 10.1037/h0025743
Gogel, 1973, Absolute motion parallax and the specific distance tendency, Percept. & Psychophy., 13, 284, 10.3758/BF03214141
Gogel, 1979, A comparison of oculomotor and motion parallax cues of egocentric distance, Vision Res., 19, 1161, 10.1016/0042-6989(79)90013-0
Goodson, 1980, Motion parallax in the perception of movement by a moving subject, Bull. Psychon. Soc., 6, 87, 10.3758/BF03334446
Graham, 1948, Factors influencing thresholds for monocular movement parallax, J. Exp. Psychol., 38, 205, 10.1037/h0054067
Gulick, 1976
Gyr, 1979, Motor-sensory feedback and geometry of visual space: An attempted replication, Behav. and Brain Sci., 2, 59, 10.1017/S0140525X00060702
Haber, 1978, Visual perception, Annu. Rev. Psychol., 29, 31, 10.1146/annurev.ps.29.020178.000335
Haber, 1979, Perceiving the layout of space in pictures: A perspective theory based on Leonardo da Vinci, 84
Haber, 1979, When is sensory-motion information necessary, when only useful, and when superfluos?, Behav. and Brain Sci., 2, 68, 10.1017/S0140525X00060787
Haber, 1980, How we perceive depth flat pictures, Amer. Sci., 68, 370
Haber, 1980, Perceiving space from pictures: A Theoretical analysis, 3
Haber, 1981, The power of visual perceiving, J. Ment. Imagery, 5, 1
Haber, 1983, Stimulus information and processing mechanisms in visual perception, 157
Haber, 1985, The control of mobility by perceiving spatial layout: Application from sighted to blind travelers
Hagen, 1981, The effect of binocular and motion-generated information on the perception of depth and height, Percept. and Psychophys., 30, 257, 10.3758/BF03214281
Hein, 1983
Hell, 1978, Movement parallax: An asymptotic function of amplitude and velocity of head movement, Vision Res., 18, 629, 10.1016/0042-6989(78)90142-6
Hell, 1977, Detectability of motion as a factor in depth perception by monocular movement parallax, Percept. and Psychophys., 22, 526, 10.3758/BF03198758
1978
Hochberg, 1978
Hochberg, 1979, Pictorial limitations on perception, 313
Hochberg, 1978, The perception of motion pictures, 259
Hochberg, 1976, Perceptual analysis of moving patterns, Science (Washington, D. C.), 194, 1081, 10.1126/science.982065
Hochberg, 1977, Feature saliency, “mentak rotation” times and the integration of successive views, Mem. and Cognit., 5, 23, 10.3758/BF03209187
Hock, 1981, Alternative spatial reference systems: Intentional vs. incidental learning, Percept. and Psychophys., 29, 467, 10.3758/BF03207360
Howard, 1982
Indow, 1974, Application of multidimensional scaling in perception, 10.1016/B978-0-12-161902-2.50021-3
Irwin, 1983, Evidence against visual integration across saccidic eye movements, Percept. and Psychophys., 34, 49, 10.3758/BF03205895
Ittelson, 1973, Environmental perception and contemporary perceptual theory
Ittelson, 1978, Environmental perception and urban experience, Environ. and Behav., 10, 193, 10.1177/0013916578102004
Johansson, 1977, Studies on the visual perception of locomotion, Perception, 6, 365, 10.1068/p060365
Jones, 1981, Why two eyes are better than one: The two views of binocular vision, J. Exp. Psychol., Human Percept. and Perform., 7, 30, 10.1037/0096-1523.7.1.30
Klein, 1977, Stereopsis and the representation of space, Perception, 6, 327, 10.1068/p060327
Koenderlink, 1976, Local structure of motion parallax of a plane, J. Opt. Soc. Amer.8, 66, 717, 10.1364/JOSA.66.000717
Kosslyn, 1974, Cognition maps in children and men, Child Dev., 45, 707, 10.2307/1127837
Krest, 1978, Memory psychophysics for visual area and length, Mem. and Cognit., 6, 327, 10.3758/BF03197463
Kruskal, 1964, Mutidimensional scaling by optimizing goodness of fit to a nonmetric hypothesis, Psychometrika, 29, 1, 10.1007/BF02289565
Kruskal, 1964, Nonmentric multidimensional scaling: a numerical method, Psychometrika, 29, 115, 10.1007/BF02289694
Kruskal, 1973
Kullhavy, 1983, Spatial representation of maps, Amer. J. Psychol., 96, 337, 10.2307/1422316
Kunnapas, 1960, Scales for subjective distance, Scand. J. Psychol., 1, 187, 10.1111/j.1467-9450.1960.tb01299.x
Lee, 1977, Visual control of locomotion, Scand. J. Psychol., 18, 224, 10.1111/j.1467-9450.1977.tb00281.x
Lee, 1982, Regulatin of gait in long jumping, J. Exp. Psychol., Human Percept. and Perform., 8, 448, 10.1037/0096-1523.8.3.448
Levine, 1982, Principles of spatial problem solving, J. Exp. Psychol. General, 111, 157, 10.1037/0096-3445.111.2.157
Lewis, 1975, Determinants of visual attention in real-world scenes, Percept. Motor Skills, 41, 411, 10.2466/pms.1975.41.2.411
Liben, 1981
Lumsden, 1983, Perception of radial distance as a function of magnification and truncation of depicted spatial layout, Percept. and Psychophysics, 33, 177, 10.3758/BF03202836
Marks, 1974
Marr, 1982
Matin, 1976, Saccades and extra-retinal signals for visual direction, 205
Michaels, 1981
Moyer, 1982, Memory psychophysics, 35
Nakayama, 1974, Optical patterns, velocity-sensitive neurons, and space perception: A hypothesis, Perception, 3, 63, 10.1068/p030063
Neisser, 1976
Olsen, 1983
Palij, 1984, The orientation of cognitive maps, Bull. Psychon. Soc., 22, 105, 10.3758/BF03333776
Pick, 1983
1982
Rayner, 1983, Is visual information integrated across saccades?, Percept. and Psychophys., 34, 39, 10.3758/BF03205894
Rock, 1977, On unconscious inference, 313
Runeson, 1977, On the possibility of “smart” perceptual mechanisms, Scand. J. Psychol., 18, 172, 10.1111/j.1467-9450.1977.tb00274.x
Shebilske, 1983, Induced esophoric shifts in eye convergence and illusory distance in reduced adn structured viewing conditions, J. Exp. Psychol., Human Percept. and Perform., 9, 270, 10.1037/0096-1523.9.2.270
Shebilske, 1981, The priority of perceived distance for perceiving motion has not been demonstrated: Critical comments on Gogel's “The sensing of retinal motion”, Percept. and Psychophys., 29, 170, 10.3758/BF03207281
Shebilske, 1984, Efferent factors in natural events can be rationalized and verified: A reply to Turvey and Solomon, J. Exp. Psychol., Human Percept. and Perform., 10, 455, 10.1037/0096-1523.10.3.455
Shepard, 1962, The analysis of proximities: Multidimensional scaling with an unknown distance function, I, Psychometrika, 27, 125, 10.1007/BF02289630
Shepard, 1962, The analysis of proximities: Multidimensional scaling with an unknown distance function, II, Psychometrika, 27, 219, 10.1007/BF02289621
Shepard, 1964, Attention and the metric structure of the stimulus space, J. Math. Psychol., 1, 54, 10.1016/0022-2496(64)90017-3
Singer, 1982, Assumed distance as a determination of apparent size, Bull. Psychon. Soc., 19, 267, 10.3758/BF03330253
Stevens, 1983, Surface tilt (the direction of slant): A neglected psychopysical variable, Percept. and Psychophys., 33, 241, 10.3758/BF03202860
Stevens, 1951
Stevens, 1974, Perceptual magnitude and its measurement, 361
Strelow, 1985, What is needed for a theory of morbility: Direct perception and cognitive maps; some lessons from the blind, Psychol. Bull., 97
Strelow, 1982, Use of foreground and background information in visually guided locomotion, Perception, 10, 191, 10.1068/p100191
Teghtsoonian, 1978, Range and regression effects in magnitude scaling, Percept. and Psychophys., 24, 305, 10.3758/BF03204247
Thorndyke, 1981, Distance estimation from cognitive maps, Cognit. Psychol., 13, 526, 10.1016/0010-0285(81)90019-0
Thorndyke, 1982, Differences in spatial knowledge acquired from maps and navigation, Cognit. Psychol., 14, 560, 10.1016/0010-0285(82)90019-6
Todd, 1981, Visual information about moving objects, J. Exp. Psychol., Human Percept. and Perform., 7, 795, 10.1037/0096-1523.7.4.795
Toye, 1984
R. Toyne, The effects of viewing position on the perceived layout of space, submitted for publication.
Turvey, 1977, Contrasting orientations to a theory of visual information processing, Psychol. Rev., 84, 67, 10.1037/0033-295X.84.1.67
Turvey, 1979, The thesis of the efference-mediation of vision cannot be rationalized, Behav. and Brain Sci., 2, 81, 10.1017/S0140525X00060982
Turvey, 1984, Visually perceiving distance: A comment on Shebilske, Kramiohl, and Proffitt (1983), J. Exp. Psychol., Human Percept. and Perform., 10, 449, 10.1037/0096-1523.10.3.449
Tversky, 1981, Distortions in memory for maps, Cognit. Psychol., 13, 407, 10.1016/0010-0285(81)90016-5
Tversky, 1983, Catagories of environmental scenes, Cognit. Psychol., 15, 121, 10.1016/0010-0285(83)90006-3
Tyer, 1983, Instruction effects on size and distance judgements, Percept. and Psychophys., 34, 135, 10.3758/BF03211338
van der Meer, 1979, Interrelation of the effects of binocular disparity and perspective dues on judgments of depth and height, Percept. and Psychophys., 26, 481, 10.3758/BF03204288
Verrillo, 1983, Stability of line-length estimates using the method of absolute magnitude estimation, Percept. and Psychopys., 33, 261, 10.3758/BF03202863
Ward, 1981, The psychological representation of molar physical environments, J. Exp. Psychol., General, 110, 121, 10.1037/0096-3445.110.2.121
Warren, 1984, Perceiving affordances: visual guidance of stair climbing, J. Exp. Psychol., Human Percept. and Perform., 10, 683, 10.1037/0096-1523.10.5.683
Wheeler, 1982, What is the role of motion parallax in perception?