Toward a theory of the perceived spatial layout of scenes

Computer Vision, Graphics, and Image Processing - Tập 31 - Trang 282-321 - 1985
Ralph Norman Haber1
1Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Chicago, Chicago, Illinois 60680, USA

Tài liệu tham khảo

Arbie, 1973, Multidimensional scaling of measures of distance between partitions, J. Math. Psychol., 10, 148, 10.1016/0022-2496(73)90012-6 Attneave, 1950, Dimensions of similarity, Amer. J. Psychol., 63, 516, 10.2307/1418869 Barrow, 1978, Recovering intrinsic scene characteristics from imagex Baum, 1979, Cognitive maps: Analysis of comparative judgments of distance, Mem. and Cognit., 7, 462, 10.3758/BF03198262 Bengston, 1980, Optic array determinants of apparent distance and size in pictures, J. Exp. Psychol., Human Percept. and Perform., 6, 751, 10.1037/0096-1523.6.4.751 Biederman, 1981, On the semantics of a glance at a scene, 213 Biederman, 1981, Do background depth gradients facilitate object identification?, Perception, 10, 573, 10.1068/p100573 Brabyn, 1977, Computer-analysed measures of characteristics of human locomotion and mobility, Behav. Res. Methods and Instrum., 9, 456, 10.3758/BF03202286 Bradley, 1984, Psychophysical functions for perceived and remembered distance, Perception, 13, 315, 10.1068/p130315 Brady, 1982 Brady, 1982, Artif. Intell., 18 Braunstein, 1981, Velocity gradients and relative depth perception, Percept. & Psychophys., 29, 145, 10.3758/BF03207278 Brown, 1937, The path of seen movement as a function of the vector-field, American J. Psychol., 44, 543, 10.2307/1416380 Byrne, 1983, Distances and directions in the cognitive map of the blind, Can. J. Psychol., 37, 293, 10.1037/h0080726 Campbell, 1971, The tilt after-effect: A fresh look, Vision Res., 11, 833, 10.1016/0042-6989(71)90005-8 J. D. Carrol and J. J. Chang, Analysis of individual differences in multi-dimensional scaling via an n-way generalization of “Eckart-Young” decomposition, Psychometrika 35,238–319. J. A. Da Silva and A. Dos Santos, The effects of instructions on scales for perceived egocentric distance in a large open field, Bull. Psychon. Sot. 22,189–192. Davidson, 1975 Degelman, 1979, Motion parallax and children's distance perception, Dev. Psychol., 15, 147, 10.1037/0012-1649.15.2.147 De Renzi, 1982 Dillon, 1983 Dolezal, 1982 Epstein, 1977 Epstein, 1964, Examination of Gibson's psychophysical hypothesis, Psychol. Bull., 62, 180, 10.1037/h0044097 Eriksson, 1974, Movement parallax during locomotion, Percept. and Psychophys., 16, 197, 10.3758/BF03203274 Farber, 1979, Optical motions as information for unsigned depth, J. Exper. Psychol., Human Percept. and Perform., 5, 494, 10.1037/0096-1523.5.3.494 Feldman, 1985, Four frames suffice: A provisional model of vision and space, Behav. and Brain Sci., 8, 10.1017/S0140525X00020707 Ferris, 1972, Motion parallax and absolute distance, J. Exp. Psychol., 95, 258, 10.1037/h0033605 Fitch, 1978, On the control of activity: Some remarks from an ecological point of view Foley, 1984, Mental mapping of a megastructure, Can. J. Psychol., 38, 440, 10.1037/h0080853 Foley, 1980, Binocular distance perception, Psychol. Rev., 87, 411, 10.1037/0033-295X.87.5.411 Foley, 1972, visually directed pointing as a function of target distance, direction and available cues, Percept. and Psychophys., 12, 263, 10.3758/BF03207201 Gibson, 1954, Motion parallax as a determinant of perceived depth, J. Exp. Psychol., 58, 40, 10.1037/h0043883 Gibson, 1950 Gibson, 1977, On the analysis of change in the optic array, Scand. J. Psychol., 18, 161, 10.1111/j.1467-9450.1977.tb00272.x Gibson, 1979 Gibson, 1952, Does motion perspective independently produce the impressing of a receding surface?, J. Exp. Psychol., 44, 16, 10.1037/h0056030 Gibson, 1955, A method for controlling stimulation for the study of space perception: The optical tunnel, J. Exp. Psych., 50, 1, 10.1037/h0043072 Gilinsky, 1951, Perceived size and distance in visual space, Psychol. Rev., 58, 460, 10.1037/h0061505 Gogel, 1963, The Visual Perception of Spatial Extent, FAA-CARI document No. 63-20, Oklahoma City Gogel, 1974, cognitive factors in spatial responses, Psychologica, 17, 213 Gogel, 1976, An indirect method of measuring perceived distance from familiar size, Percept. and Psychophys., 20, 419, 10.3758/BF03208276 Gogel, 1979, Depth adjacency and induced motion, Percept. and Motor Skills, 48, 343, 10.2466/pms.1979.48.2.343 Gogel, 1968, Perceived depth between familiar objects, J. Exp. Psychol., 77, 206, 10.1037/h0025743 Gogel, 1973, Absolute motion parallax and the specific distance tendency, Percept. & Psychophy., 13, 284, 10.3758/BF03214141 Gogel, 1979, A comparison of oculomotor and motion parallax cues of egocentric distance, Vision Res., 19, 1161, 10.1016/0042-6989(79)90013-0 Goodson, 1980, Motion parallax in the perception of movement by a moving subject, Bull. Psychon. Soc., 6, 87, 10.3758/BF03334446 Graham, 1948, Factors influencing thresholds for monocular movement parallax, J. Exp. Psychol., 38, 205, 10.1037/h0054067 Gulick, 1976 Gyr, 1979, Motor-sensory feedback and geometry of visual space: An attempted replication, Behav. and Brain Sci., 2, 59, 10.1017/S0140525X00060702 Haber, 1978, Visual perception, Annu. Rev. Psychol., 29, 31, 10.1146/annurev.ps.29.020178.000335 Haber, 1979, Perceiving the layout of space in pictures: A perspective theory based on Leonardo da Vinci, 84 Haber, 1979, When is sensory-motion information necessary, when only useful, and when superfluos?, Behav. and Brain Sci., 2, 68, 10.1017/S0140525X00060787 Haber, 1980, How we perceive depth flat pictures, Amer. Sci., 68, 370 Haber, 1980, Perceiving space from pictures: A Theoretical analysis, 3 Haber, 1981, The power of visual perceiving, J. Ment. Imagery, 5, 1 Haber, 1983, Stimulus information and processing mechanisms in visual perception, 157 Haber, 1985, The control of mobility by perceiving spatial layout: Application from sighted to blind travelers Hagen, 1981, The effect of binocular and motion-generated information on the perception of depth and height, Percept. and Psychophys., 30, 257, 10.3758/BF03214281 Hein, 1983 Hell, 1978, Movement parallax: An asymptotic function of amplitude and velocity of head movement, Vision Res., 18, 629, 10.1016/0042-6989(78)90142-6 Hell, 1977, Detectability of motion as a factor in depth perception by monocular movement parallax, Percept. and Psychophys., 22, 526, 10.3758/BF03198758 1978 Hochberg, 1978 Hochberg, 1979, Pictorial limitations on perception, 313 Hochberg, 1978, The perception of motion pictures, 259 Hochberg, 1976, Perceptual analysis of moving patterns, Science (Washington, D. C.), 194, 1081, 10.1126/science.982065 Hochberg, 1977, Feature saliency, “mentak rotation” times and the integration of successive views, Mem. and Cognit., 5, 23, 10.3758/BF03209187 Hock, 1981, Alternative spatial reference systems: Intentional vs. incidental learning, Percept. and Psychophys., 29, 467, 10.3758/BF03207360 Howard, 1982 Indow, 1974, Application of multidimensional scaling in perception, 10.1016/B978-0-12-161902-2.50021-3 Irwin, 1983, Evidence against visual integration across saccidic eye movements, Percept. and Psychophys., 34, 49, 10.3758/BF03205895 Ittelson, 1973, Environmental perception and contemporary perceptual theory Ittelson, 1978, Environmental perception and urban experience, Environ. and Behav., 10, 193, 10.1177/0013916578102004 Johansson, 1977, Studies on the visual perception of locomotion, Perception, 6, 365, 10.1068/p060365 Jones, 1981, Why two eyes are better than one: The two views of binocular vision, J. Exp. Psychol., Human Percept. and Perform., 7, 30, 10.1037/0096-1523.7.1.30 Klein, 1977, Stereopsis and the representation of space, Perception, 6, 327, 10.1068/p060327 Koenderlink, 1976, Local structure of motion parallax of a plane, J. Opt. Soc. Amer.8, 66, 717, 10.1364/JOSA.66.000717 Kosslyn, 1974, Cognition maps in children and men, Child Dev., 45, 707, 10.2307/1127837 Krest, 1978, Memory psychophysics for visual area and length, Mem. and Cognit., 6, 327, 10.3758/BF03197463 Kruskal, 1964, Mutidimensional scaling by optimizing goodness of fit to a nonmetric hypothesis, Psychometrika, 29, 1, 10.1007/BF02289565 Kruskal, 1964, Nonmentric multidimensional scaling: a numerical method, Psychometrika, 29, 115, 10.1007/BF02289694 Kruskal, 1973 Kullhavy, 1983, Spatial representation of maps, Amer. J. Psychol., 96, 337, 10.2307/1422316 Kunnapas, 1960, Scales for subjective distance, Scand. J. Psychol., 1, 187, 10.1111/j.1467-9450.1960.tb01299.x Lee, 1977, Visual control of locomotion, Scand. J. Psychol., 18, 224, 10.1111/j.1467-9450.1977.tb00281.x Lee, 1982, Regulatin of gait in long jumping, J. Exp. Psychol., Human Percept. and Perform., 8, 448, 10.1037/0096-1523.8.3.448 Levine, 1982, Principles of spatial problem solving, J. Exp. Psychol. General, 111, 157, 10.1037/0096-3445.111.2.157 Lewis, 1975, Determinants of visual attention in real-world scenes, Percept. Motor Skills, 41, 411, 10.2466/pms.1975.41.2.411 Liben, 1981 Lumsden, 1983, Perception of radial distance as a function of magnification and truncation of depicted spatial layout, Percept. and Psychophysics, 33, 177, 10.3758/BF03202836 Marks, 1974 Marr, 1982 Matin, 1976, Saccades and extra-retinal signals for visual direction, 205 Michaels, 1981 Moyer, 1982, Memory psychophysics, 35 Nakayama, 1974, Optical patterns, velocity-sensitive neurons, and space perception: A hypothesis, Perception, 3, 63, 10.1068/p030063 Neisser, 1976 Olsen, 1983 Palij, 1984, The orientation of cognitive maps, Bull. Psychon. Soc., 22, 105, 10.3758/BF03333776 Pick, 1983 1982 Rayner, 1983, Is visual information integrated across saccades?, Percept. and Psychophys., 34, 39, 10.3758/BF03205894 Rock, 1977, On unconscious inference, 313 Runeson, 1977, On the possibility of “smart” perceptual mechanisms, Scand. J. Psychol., 18, 172, 10.1111/j.1467-9450.1977.tb00274.x Shebilske, 1983, Induced esophoric shifts in eye convergence and illusory distance in reduced adn structured viewing conditions, J. Exp. Psychol., Human Percept. and Perform., 9, 270, 10.1037/0096-1523.9.2.270 Shebilske, 1981, The priority of perceived distance for perceiving motion has not been demonstrated: Critical comments on Gogel's “The sensing of retinal motion”, Percept. and Psychophys., 29, 170, 10.3758/BF03207281 Shebilske, 1984, Efferent factors in natural events can be rationalized and verified: A reply to Turvey and Solomon, J. Exp. Psychol., Human Percept. and Perform., 10, 455, 10.1037/0096-1523.10.3.455 Shepard, 1962, The analysis of proximities: Multidimensional scaling with an unknown distance function, I, Psychometrika, 27, 125, 10.1007/BF02289630 Shepard, 1962, The analysis of proximities: Multidimensional scaling with an unknown distance function, II, Psychometrika, 27, 219, 10.1007/BF02289621 Shepard, 1964, Attention and the metric structure of the stimulus space, J. Math. Psychol., 1, 54, 10.1016/0022-2496(64)90017-3 Singer, 1982, Assumed distance as a determination of apparent size, Bull. Psychon. Soc., 19, 267, 10.3758/BF03330253 Stevens, 1983, Surface tilt (the direction of slant): A neglected psychopysical variable, Percept. and Psychophys., 33, 241, 10.3758/BF03202860 Stevens, 1951 Stevens, 1974, Perceptual magnitude and its measurement, 361 Strelow, 1985, What is needed for a theory of morbility: Direct perception and cognitive maps; some lessons from the blind, Psychol. Bull., 97 Strelow, 1982, Use of foreground and background information in visually guided locomotion, Perception, 10, 191, 10.1068/p100191 Teghtsoonian, 1978, Range and regression effects in magnitude scaling, Percept. and Psychophys., 24, 305, 10.3758/BF03204247 Thorndyke, 1981, Distance estimation from cognitive maps, Cognit. Psychol., 13, 526, 10.1016/0010-0285(81)90019-0 Thorndyke, 1982, Differences in spatial knowledge acquired from maps and navigation, Cognit. Psychol., 14, 560, 10.1016/0010-0285(82)90019-6 Todd, 1981, Visual information about moving objects, J. Exp. Psychol., Human Percept. and Perform., 7, 795, 10.1037/0096-1523.7.4.795 Toye, 1984 R. Toyne, The effects of viewing position on the perceived layout of space, submitted for publication. Turvey, 1977, Contrasting orientations to a theory of visual information processing, Psychol. Rev., 84, 67, 10.1037/0033-295X.84.1.67 Turvey, 1979, The thesis of the efference-mediation of vision cannot be rationalized, Behav. and Brain Sci., 2, 81, 10.1017/S0140525X00060982 Turvey, 1984, Visually perceiving distance: A comment on Shebilske, Kramiohl, and Proffitt (1983), J. Exp. Psychol., Human Percept. and Perform., 10, 449, 10.1037/0096-1523.10.3.449 Tversky, 1981, Distortions in memory for maps, Cognit. Psychol., 13, 407, 10.1016/0010-0285(81)90016-5 Tversky, 1983, Catagories of environmental scenes, Cognit. Psychol., 15, 121, 10.1016/0010-0285(83)90006-3 Tyer, 1983, Instruction effects on size and distance judgements, Percept. and Psychophys., 34, 135, 10.3758/BF03211338 van der Meer, 1979, Interrelation of the effects of binocular disparity and perspective dues on judgments of depth and height, Percept. and Psychophys., 26, 481, 10.3758/BF03204288 Verrillo, 1983, Stability of line-length estimates using the method of absolute magnitude estimation, Percept. and Psychopys., 33, 261, 10.3758/BF03202863 Ward, 1981, The psychological representation of molar physical environments, J. Exp. Psychol., General, 110, 121, 10.1037/0096-3445.110.2.121 Warren, 1984, Perceiving affordances: visual guidance of stair climbing, J. Exp. Psychol., Human Percept. and Perform., 10, 683, 10.1037/0096-1523.10.5.683 Wheeler, 1982, What is the role of motion parallax in perception?