Timing and time perception: A review of recent behavioral and neuroscience findings and theoretical directions
Tóm tắt
Từ khóa
Tài liệu tham khảo
Alexander, I., Cowey, A., & Walsh, V. (2005). The right parietal cortex and time perception: Back to Critchley and the Zeitraffer phenomenon. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 22, 306–315. doi:10.1080/02643290442000356
Allan, L. G., & Church, R. M. (EDS.) (2002). Special issue honoring the career of Professor John Gibbon. Learning & Motivation, 33(1). doi:10.1006/lmot.2001.1114
Allan, L. G., & Gerhardt, K. (2001). Temporal bisection with trial referents. Perception & Psychophysics, 63, 524–540.
Allan, L. G., & Kristofferson, A. B. (1974). Psychophysical theories of duration discrimination. Perception & Psychophysics, 16, 26–34.
Allan, L. G., Kristofferson, A. B., & Wiens, E. W. (1971). Duration discrimination of brief light flashes. Perception & Psychophysics, 9, 327–334.
Angrilli, A., Cherubini, P., Pavese, A., & Mantredini, S. (1997). The influence of affective factors on time perception. Perception & Psychophysics, 59, 972–982.
Arao, H., Suetomi, D., & Nakajima, Y. (2000). Does time-shrinking take place in visual temporal patterns? Perception, 29, 819–830. doi:10.1068/p2853
Barnes, R., & Jones, M. R. (2000). Expectancy, attention, and time. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 254–311. doi:10.1006/cogp.2000.0738
Belin, P., McAdams, S., Thivard, L., Smith, B., Savel, S., Zilbovicius, M., et al. (2002). The neuroanatomical substrate of sound duration discrimination. Neuropsychologia, 40, 1956–1964. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00062-3
Bendixen, A., Grimm, S., & Schroger, E. (2006). The relation between onset, offset, and duration perception as examined by psychophysical data and event-related brain potentials. Journal of Psychophysiology, 20, 40–51. doi:10.1027/0269-8803.20.1.40
Bindra, D., & Waksberg, H. (1956). Methods and terminology in studies of time estimation. Psychological Bulletin, 53, 155–159. doi:10.1037/h0041810
Bisson, N., Tobin, S., & Grondin, S. (2009). Remembering the duration of joyful and sad musical excerpts. NeuroQuantology, 7, 46–57.
Block, R. A. (1990). Cognitive models of psychological time. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Block, R. A. (2003). Psychological timing without a timer: The roles of attention and memory. In H. Helfrich (Ed.), Time and mind II (pp. 41–60). Göttingen: Hogrefe & Huber.
Block, R. A., & Zakay, D. (1997). Prospective and retrospective duration judgments: A meta-analytic review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4, 184–197.
Block, R. A., & Zakay, D. (2008). Timing and remembering the past, the present, and the future. In S. Grondin (Ed.), Psychology of time (pp. 367–394). Bingley, U.K.: Emerald Group.
Block, R. A., Zakay, D., & Hancock, P. A. (1999). Developmental changes in human duration judgments: A meta-analytic review. Developmental Review, 19, 183–211. doi:10.1006/drev.1998.0475
Boets, B., Wouters, J., Van Wieringen, A., & Ghesquière, P. (2006). Auditory temporal information processing in preschool children at family risk for dyslexia: Relations with phonological abilities and developing literacy skills. Brain & Language, 97, 64–79. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2005.07.026
Boltz, M. G. (1992). The remembering of auditory event durations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 18, 938–956. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.18.5.938
Boltz, M. G. (1994). Changes in internal tempo and effects on the learning and remembering of event durations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 20, 1154–1171. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.20.5.1154
Boltz, M. G. (1995). Effects of event structure on retrospective duration judgments. Perception & Psychophysics, 57, 1080–1096.
Boltz, M. G. (2005). Duration judgments of naturalistic events in the auditory and visual modalities. Perception & Psychophysics, 67, 1362–1375.
Braitenberg, V. (1967). Is the cerebellar cortex a biological clock in the millisecond range? Progress in Brain Research, 25, 334–346.
Brown, S. W. (1997). Attentional resources in timing: Interference effects in concurrent temporal and nontemporal working memory tasks. Perception & Psychophysics, 59, 1118–1140.
Brown, S. W. (2006). Timing and executive function: Bidirectional interference between concurrent temporal production and randomization tasks. Memory & Cognition, 34, 1464–1471.
Brown, S. W. (2008). Time and attention: Review of the literature. In S. Grondin (Ed.), Psychology of time (pp. 111–138). Bingley, U.K.: Emerald Group.
Brown, S. W., & Boltz, M. (2002). Attentional processes in time perception: Effects of mental workload and event structure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 28, 600–615. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.28.3.600
Brown, S. W., & Merchant, S. M. (2007). Processing resources in timing and sequencing tasks. Perception & Psychophysics, 69, 439–449.
Brown, S. W., & Stubbs, D. A. (1988). The psychophysics of retrospective and prospective timing. Perception, 17, 297–310. doi:10.1068/p170297
Brown, S. W., Stubbs, D. A., & West, A. N. (1992). Attention, multiple timing, and psychophysical scaling of temporal judgments. In F. Macar, V. Pouthas & W. J. Friedman (Eds.), Time, action, and cognition: Towards bridging the gap (pp. 129–140). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer.
Brown, S. W., & West, A. N. (1990). Multiple timing and the allocation of attention. Acta Psychologica, 75, 103–121. doi:10.1016/0001-6918(90)90081-P
Buccheri, R., Saniga, M., & Stuckey, M. (EDS.) (2003). The nature of time: Geometry, physics and perception. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer.
Bueti, D., Bahrami, B., & Walsh, V. (2008). Sensory and associative cortex in time perception. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 1054–1062. doi:10.1162/jocn.2008.20060
Bueti, D., Walsh, V., Frith, C., & Rees, G. (2008). Different brain circuits underlie motor and perceptual representations of temporal intervals. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 204–214. doi:10.1162/jocn.2008.20017
Buhusi, C. V., & Meck, W. H. (2005). What makes us tick? Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, 755–765. doi:10.1038/nrn1764
Buhusi, C. V., & Meck, W. H. (2009). Relative time sharing: New findings and an extension of the resource allocation model of temporal processing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 364, 1875–1885.
Buonomano, D. V. (2007). The biology of time across different scales. Nature Chemical Biology, 3, 594–597.
Burle, B., & Casini, L. (2001). Dissociation between activation and attention effects in time estimation: Implications for internal clock models. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 27, 195–205. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.27.1.195
Burr, D., Tozzi, A., & Morrone, M. C. (2007). Neural mechanisms for timing visual events are spatially selective in real-world coordinates. Nature Neuroscience, 10, 423–425. doi:10.1038/nn1874
Carroll, C. A., O’Donnell, B. F., Shekhar, A., & Hetrick, W. P. (2009). Timing dysfunctions in schizophrenia span from millisecond to several-second durations. Brain & Cognition, 70, 181–190. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2009.02.001
Caruso, E. M., Gilbert, D. T., & Wilson, T. D. (2008). A wrinkle in time: Asymmetric valuation of past and future events. Psychological Science, 19, 796–801. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02159.x
Casini, L., & Macar, F. (1997). Effects of attention manipulation on judgments of duration and of intensity in the visual modality. Memory & Cognition, 25, 812–818.
Chambon, M., Gil, S., Niedenthal, P. M., & Droit-Volet, S. (2005). Psychologie sociale et perception du temps: l’estimation temporelle des stimuli sociaux et émotionnels [Social psychology and time perception: The temporal estimation of social and emotional stimuli]. Psychologie Française, 50, 167–180. doi:10.1016/j.psfr.2004.10.008
Church, R. M. (1997). Timing and temporal search. In C. M. Bradshaw & E. Szabadi (Eds.), Time and behavior: Psychological and neurobehavioral analyses (pp. 41–78). Amsterdam: Elsevier, North-Holland.
Church, R. M. (2003). A concise introduction to the scalar timing theory. In W. H. Meck (Ed.), Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing (pp. 3–22). Boca Raton, FL: CRC.
Church, R. M., & Broadbent, H. A. (1990). Alternative representations of time, number, and rate. Cognition, 37, 55–81. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(90)90018-F
Coull, J. T., Vidal, F., Nazarian, B., & Macar, F. (2004). Functional anatomy of the attentional modulation of time estimation. Science, 303, 1506–1508.
Creelman, C. D. (1962). Human discrimination of auditory duration. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 34, 582–593. doi:10.1121/1.1918172
Crystal, J. D. (ED.) (2007). The psychology of time: A tribute to the contributions of Russell M. Church [Special Issue]. Behavioural Processes, 74(2).
Danckert, J., Ferber, S., Pun, C., Broderick, C., Striemer, C., Rock, S., & Stewart, D. (2007). Neglected time: Impaired temporal perception of multisecond intervals in unilateral neglect. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 1706–1720. doi:10.1162/jocn.2007.19.10.1706
Davalos, D. B., Kisley, M. A., & Freedman, R. (2005). Behavioral and electrophysiological indices of temporal processing dysfunction in schizophrenia. Journal of Neuropsychiatry & Clinical Neurosciences, 17, 517–525.
Davalos, D. B., Kisley, M. A., Polk, S. D., & Ross, R. G. (2003). Mismatch negativity in detection of interval duration deviation in schizophrenia. Cognitive Neuroscience & Neuropsychology, 14, 1283–1286. doi:10.1097/00001756-200307010-00019
Davalos, D. B., Kisley, M. A., & Ross, R. G. (2002). Deficits in auditory and visual temporal perception in schizophrenia. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 7, 273–282. doi:10.1080/13546800143000230
Davalos, D. B., Kisley, M. A., & Ross, R. G. (2003). Effects of interval duration on temporal processing in schizophrenia. Brain & Cognition, 52, 295–301. doi:10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00157-X
Drake, C., & Botte, M.-C. (1993). Tempo sensitivity in auditory sequences: Evidence for a multiple-look model. Perception & Psychophysics, 54, 277–286.
Droit-Volet, S., Brunot, S., & Niedenthal, P. M. (2004). Perception of the duration of emotional events. Cognition & Emotion, 18, 849–858. doi:10.1080/02699930341000194
Droit-Volet, S., & Gil, S. (2009). Emotion and time perception. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 364, 1943–1953.
Droit-Volet, S., & Meck, W. H. (2007). How emotions colour our perception of time. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 504–513. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2007.09.008
Droit-Volet, S., Meck, W. H., & Penney, T. B. (2007). Sensory modality and time perception in children and adults. Behavioural Processes, 74, 244–250. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2006.09.012
Droit-Volet, S., Wearden, J. H., & Delgado, M. D. (2007). Shortterm memory for time in children and adults: A behavioral study and a model. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 97, 246–264. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2007.02.003
Eagleman, D. M. (2008). Human time perception and its illusions. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 18, 131–136. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2008.06.002
Edwards, V. T., Giaschi, D. E., Dougherty, R. F., Edgell, D., Bjornson, B. H., Lyons, C., & Douglas, R. M. (2004). Psychophysical indexes of temporal processing abnormalities in children with developmental dyslexia. Developmental Neuropsychology, 25, 321–354. doi:10.1207/s15326942dn2503_5
Effron, D. A., Niedenthal, P. M., Gil, S., & Droit-Volet, S. (2006). Embodied temporal perception of emotion. Emotion, 6, 1–9. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.6.1.1
Eisler, A. D., Eisler, H., & Montgomery, H. (2004). A quantitative model for retrospective subjective duration. NeuroQuantology, 4, 263–291.
Eisler, H. (1975). Subjective duration and psychophysics. Psychological Review, 82, 429–450. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.82.6.429
Eisler, H. (1976). Experiments on subjective duration 1878–1975: A collection of power function exponents. Psychological Bulletin, 83, 1154–1171. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.83.6.1154
Eisler, H. (2003). The parallel-clock model: A tool for quantification of experienced duration. In R. Buccheri, M. Saniga, & M. Stuckey (Eds.), The nature of time: Geometry, physics and perception (pp. 19–26). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer.
Eisler, H., & Eisler, A. D. (1992). Time perception: Effects of sex and sound intensity on scales of subjective duration. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 33, 339–358. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9450.1992.tb00923.x
Eisler, H., Eisler, A. D., & Hellström, Å. (2008). Psychophysical issues in the study of time perception. In S. Grondin (Ed.), Psychology of time (pp. 75–110). Bingley, U.K.: Emerald Group.
Elvevåg, B., Brown, G. D. A., McCormack, T., Vousden, J. I., & Goldberg, T. E. (2004). Identification of tone duration, line length, and letter position: An experimental approach to timing and working memory deficits in schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113, 509–521. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.113.4.509
Elvevåg, B., McCormack, T., Gilbert, A., Brown, G. D. A., Weinberger, D. R., & Goldberg, T. E. (2003). Duration judgments in patients with schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 33, 1249–1261.
Ferrandez, A. M., Hugueville, L., Lehericy, S., Poline, J. B., Marsault, C., & Pouthas, V. (2003). Basal ganglia and supplementary motor area subtend duration perception: An fMRI study. NeuroImage, 19, 1532–1544.
Field, D. T., & Groeger, J. A. (2004). Temporal interval production and short-term memory. Perception & Psychophysics, 66, 808–819.
Ford, M. P., Wagenaar, R. C., & Newell, K. M. (2007). The effects of auditory rhythms and instruction on walking patterns in individuals post stroke. Gait & Posture, 26, 150–155.
Fortin, C., Bédard, M.-C., & Champagne, J. (2005). Timing during interruptions in timing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 31, 276–288. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.31.2.276
Fortin, C., Fairhurst, S., Malapani, C., Morin, C., Towey, J., & Meck, W. H. (2009). Expectancy in humans in multisecond peakinterval timing with gaps. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71, 789–802.
Fraisse, P. (1956). Les structures rythmiques [Rhythmic structures]. Louvain, Belgium: Studia Psychologica.
Fraisse, P. (1957). Psychologie du temps [Psychology of time]. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.
Fraisse, P. (1978). Time and rhythm perception. In E. Carterette & M. Friedman (Eds.), Handbook of perception: Vol. 8. Perceptual coding (pp. 203–254). New York: Academic Press.
Frassinetti, F., Magnani, B., & Oliveri, M. (2009). Prismatic lenses shift time perception. Psychological Science, 20, 949–954. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02390.x
Friberg, A., & Sundberg, J. (1995). Time discrimination in a monotonic, isochronic sequence. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 98, 2524–2531. doi:10.1121/1.413218
Friedman, W. J. (1993). Memory for the time of past events. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 44–66. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.113.1.44
Friedman, W. J. (2008). Developmental perspectives on the psychology of time. In S. Grondin (Ed.), Psychology of time (pp. 345–366). Bingley, U.K.: Emerald Group.
Gamache, P.-L., & Grondin, S. (2008). Temporal limits of memory for time. In B. Schneider, B. M. Ben-David, S. Parker, & W. Wong (Eds.), Fechner Day 2008: Proceedings of the 24th Annual Meeting of the ISP (pp. 173–178). Toronto: The ISP.
Gibbon, J. (1977). Scalar expectancy theory and Weber’s law in animal timing. Psychological Review, 84, 279–325.
Gibbon, J. (1991). Origins of scalar timing. Learning & Motivation, 22, 3–38. doi:10.1016/0023-9690(91)90015-Z
Gibbon, J. (1992). Ubiquity of scalar timing with a Poisson clock. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 36, 283–293. doi:10.1016/0022-2496(92)90041-5
Gibbon, J., Church, R. M., & Meck, W. H. (1984). Scalar timing in memory. In J. Gibbon & L. Allan (Eds.), Timing and time perception (Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, Vol. 423, pp. 52–77). New York: New York Academy of Sciences.
Gil, S., Niedenthal, P. M., & Droit-Volet, S. (2007). Anger and time perception in children. Emotion, 7, 219–225. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.7.1.219
Gilden, D. L., & Marusich, L. R. (2009). Contraction of time in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychology, 23, 265–269. doi:10.1037/a0014553
Glicksohn, J. (2001). Temporal cognition and the phenomenology of time: A multiplicative function for apparent duration. Consciousness & Cognition, 10, 1–25. doi:10.1006/ccog.2000.0468
Glicksohn, J., & Cohen, Y. (2000). Can music alleviate cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia? Psychopathology, 33, 43–47. doi:10.1159/000029118
Glicksohn, J., & Myslobodsky, M. S. (EDS.) (2006). Timing the future: The case for a time-based prospective memory. London: World Scientific Publishing.
Goldreich, D. (2007). A Bayesian perceptual model replicates the cutaneous rabbit and other tactile spatiotemporal illusions. PLoS ONE, 2, e333. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0000333
Goldstone, S., & Lhamon, W. T. (1974). Studies of auditory-visual differences in human time judgment: I. Sounds are judged longer than lights. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 39, 63–82.
Grondin, S. (1993). Duration discrimination of empty and filled intervals marked by auditory and visual signals. Perception & Psychophysics, 54, 383–394.
Grondin, S. (2001a). Discriminating time intervals presented in sequences marked by visual signals. Perception & Psychophysics, 63, 1214–1228.
Grondin, S. (2001b). From physical time to the first and second moments of psychological time. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 22–44. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.127.1.22
Grondin, S. (2001c). A temporal account of the limited processing capacity. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 24, 122–123.
Grondin, S. (2003). Sensory modalities and temporal processing. In H. Helfrich (Ed.), Time and mind II (pp. 61–77). Göttingen: Hogrefe & Huber.
Grondin, S. (2005). Overloading temporal memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 31, 869–879. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.31.5.869
Grondin, S. (2008a). Methods for studying psychological time. In S. Grondin (Ed.), Psychology of time (pp. 51–74). Bingley, U.K.: Emerald Group.
Grondin, S. (ED.) (2008b). Psychology of time. Bingley, U.K.: Emerald Group.
Grondin, S., Bisson, N., Gagnon, C., Gamache, P.-L., & Matteau, A.-A. (2009). Little to be expected from auditory training for improving visual temporal discrimination. NeuroQuantology, 7, 95–102.
Grondin, S., Dionne, G., Malenfant, N., Plourde, M., Cloutier, M.-E., & Jean, C. (2007). Temporal processing skills of children with and without specific language impairment. Canadian Journal of Speech-Language Pathology & Audiology, 31, 38–46.
Grondin, S., Gamache, P.-L., Tobin, S., Bisson, N., & Hawke, L. (2008). Categorization of brief temporal intervals: An auditory processing context may impair visual performances. Acoustical Science & Technology, 29, 338–340.
Grondin, S., & Killeen, P. R. (2009). Tracking time with song and count: Different Weber functions for musicians and nonmusicians. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71, 1649–1654.
Grondin, S., & Macar, F. (1992). Dividing attention between temporal and nontemporal tasks: A performance operating characteristic— POC—analysis. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum.
Grondin, S., & McAuley, J. D. (2009). Duration discrimination in crossmodal sequences. Perception, 38, 1542–1559.
Grondin, S., Meilleur-Wells, G., & Lachance, R. (1999). When to start explicit counting in a time-intervals discrimination task: Acritical point in the timing process of humans. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 25, 993–1004. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.25.4.993
Grondin, S., Ouellet, B., & Roussel, M.-E. (2004). Benefits and limits of explicit counting for discriminating temporal intervals. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 1–12. doi:10.1037/h0087436
Grondin, S., & Plourde, M. (2007a). Discrimination of time intervals presented in sequences: Spatial effects with multiple auditory sources. Human Movement Science, 26, 702–716. doi:10.1016/j.humov.2007.07.009
Grondin, S., & Plourde, M. (2007b). Judging multi-minute intervals retrospectively. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 1303–1312. doi:10.1080/17470210600988976
Grondin, S., Pouthas, V., Samson, S., & Roy, M. (2006). Mécanismes et désordres liés à l’adaptation au temps [Mechanisms and disorders related to the adaptation to time]. Canadian Psychology, 47, 170–183. doi:10.1037/cp2006007
Grondin, S., & Rammsayer, T. (2003). Variable foreperiods and temporal discrimination. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56A, 731–765. doi:10.1080/02724980244000611
Grondin, S., & Rousseau, R. (1991). Judging the relative duration of multimodal short empty time intervals. Perception & Psychophysics, 49, 245–256.
Grondin, S., Roussel, M.-E., Gamache, P.-L., Roy, M., & Ouellet, B. (2005). The structure of sensory events and the accuracy of time judgments. Perception, 34, 45–58. doi:10.1068/p5369
Guillot, A., & Collet, C. (2005). Duration of mentally simulated movement: A review. Journal of Motor Behavior, 37, 10–20. doi:10.3200/JMBR.37.1.10-20
Handy, T. C., Gazzaniga, M. S., & Ivry, R. B. (2003). Cortical and subcortical contributions to the representation of temporal information. Neuropsychologia, 41, 1461–1473. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(03)00093-9
Harrington, D. L., & Haaland, K. Y. (1999). Neural underpinnings of temporal processing: A review of focal lesion, pharmacological, and functional imaging research. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 10, 91–116.
Harrington, D. L., Lee, R. R., Boyd, L. A., Rapcsak, S. Z., & Knight, R. T. (2004). Does the representation of time depend on the cerebellum? Effect of cerebellar stroke. Brain, 127, 1–14. doi:10.1093/brain/awh065
Hecht, H., & Savelsbergh, G. (Eds.) (2004). Time-to-contact (Advances in Psychology, Vol. 135). Amsterdam: Elsevier, North-Holland.
Helfrich, H. (ED.) (2003). Time and mind II: Information processing perspectives. Seattle: Hogrefe & Huber.
Hellström, Å. (1985). The time-order error and its relatives: Mirrors of cognitive processes in comparing. Psychological Bulletin, 97, 35–61. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.97.1.35
Hellström, Å., & Rammsayer, T. H. (2004). Effects of time-order, interstimulus interval, and feedback in duration discrimination of noise bursts in the 50- and 1000-ms ranges. Acta Psychologica, 116, 1–20. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2003.11.003
Helson, H. (1964). Adaptation-level theory. New York: Harper & Row.
Hemmes, N. S., Brown, B. L., & Kladopoulos, C. N. (2004). Time perception with and without a concurrent nontemporal task. Perception & Psychophysics, 66, 328–341.
Henry, M. J., & McAuley, J. D. (2009). Evaluation of an imputed pitch velocity model of the auditory kappa effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 35, 551–564. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.35.2.551
Henry, M. J., McAuley, J. D., & Zaleha, M. (2009). Perceived pitch depends on perceived time: Further test of an auditory motion hypothesis. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71, 1399–1413.
Hicks, R. E., Miller, G. W., & Kinsbourne, M. (1976). Prospective and retrospective judgments of time as a function of amount of information processed. American Journal of Psychology, 89, 719–730. doi:10.2307/1421469
Hinton, S. C., Harrington, D. L., Binder, J. R., Durgerian, S., & Rao, S. M. (2004). Neural systems supporting timing and chronometric counting: An FMRI study. Cognitive Brain Research, 21, 183–192. doi:10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.04.009
Hinton, S. C., & Meck, W. H. (2004). Frontal-striatal circuitry activated by human peak-interval timing in the supra-seconds range. Cognitive Brain Research, 21, 171–182. doi:10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.08.005
Hinton, S. C., & Rao, S. M. (2004). “One-thousand one. . . onethousand two . . .”: Chronometric counting violates the scalar property in interval timing. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 24–30.
Hirsh, I. J., Monahan, C. B., Grant, K. W., & Singh, P. G. (1990). Studies in auditory timing: 1. Simple patterns. Perception & Psychophysics, 47, 215–226.
Hirsh, I. J., & Sherrick, C. E. (1961). Perceived order in different sense modalities. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 423–432. doi:10.1037/h0045283
Hodinott-Hill, I., Thilo, K. V., Cowey, A., & Walsh, V. (2002). Auditory chronostasis: Hanging on the telephone. Current Biology, 12, 1779–1781. doi:10.1016/S0960-9822(02)01219-8
Hopson, J. W. (2003). General learning models: Timing without a clock. In W. H. Meck (Ed.), Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing (pp. 23–60). Boca Raton, FL: CRC.
Hornik, J. (1992). Time estimation and orientation mediated by transient mood. Journal of Socio-Economics, 21, 209–227. doi:10.1016/1053-5357(92)90010-5
Ivry, R. B., & Hazeltine, R. E. (1995). The perception and production of temporal intervals across a range of durations: Evidence for a common timing mechanism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 21, 3–18. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.21.1.3
Ivry, R. B., & Keele, S. W. (1989). Timing functions of the cerebellum. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 1, 136–152. doi:10.1162/jocn.1989.1.2.136
Ivry, R. B., Keele, S. W., & Diener, H. C. (1988). Dissociation of the lateral and medial cerebellum in movement timing and movement execution. Experimental Brain Research, 73, 167–180.
Ivry, R. B., & Richardson, T. C. (2002). Temporal control and coordination: The multiple timer model. Brain & Cognition, 48, 117–132. doi:10.1006/brcg.2001.1308
Ivry, R. B., & Schlerf, J. (2008). Dedicated and intrinsic models of time perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 273–280. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2008.04.002
Jahanshahi, M., Jones, C. R. G., Dirnberger, G., & Frith, C. D. (2006). The substantia nigra pars compacta and temporal processing. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 12266–12273. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2540-06.2006
Jantzen, K. J., Steinberg, F. L., & Kelso, J. A. S. (2005). Functional MRI reveals the existence of modality and coordination-dependent timing networks. NeuroImage, 25, 1031–1042.
Johnston, A., Arnold, D. H., & Nishida, S. (2006). Spatially localized distortions of event time. Current Biology, 16, 472–479.
Jones, B., & Huang, Y. L. (1982). Space-time dependencies in psychophysical judgment of extent and duration: Algebraic models of the tau and kappa effects. Psychological Bulletin, 91, 128–142. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.91.1.128
Jones, L. A., & Wearden, J. H. (2003). More is not necessarily better: Examining the nature of the temporal reference memory component in timing. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56B, 321–343. doi:10.1080/02724990244000287
Jones, L. A., & Wearden, J. H. (2004). Double standards: Memory loading in temporal reference memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57B, 55–77. doi:10.1080/02724990344000088
Jones, M. R. (1976). Time, our lost dimension: Toward a new theory of perception, attention, and memory. Psychological Review, 83, 323–355. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.83.5.323
Jones, M. R., & Boltz, M. (1989). Dynamic attending and responses to time. Psychological Review, 96, 459–491. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.96.3.459
Jones, M. R., Johnston, H. M., & Puente, J. (2006). Effects of auditory pattern structure on anticipatory and reactive attending. Cognitive Psychology, 53, 59–96. doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2006.01.003
Jones, M. R., & McAuley, J. D. (2005). Time judgments in global temporal contexts. Perception & Psychophysics, 67, 398–417.
Jueptner, M., Rijntjes, M., Weiller, C., Faiss, J. H., Timmann, D., Mueller, S. P., & Diener, H. C. (1995). Localization of a cerebellar timing process using PET. Neurology, 45, 1540–1545.
Kanai, R., & Watanabe, M. (2006). Visual onset expands subjective time. Perception & Psychophysics, 68, 1113–1123.
Karmarkar, U. R., & Buonomano, D. V. (2007). Timing in the absence of clocks: Encoding time in neural network states. Neuron, 53, 427–438.
Keele, S. W., & Ivry, R. B. (1991). Does the cerebellum provide a common computation for diverse tasks? A timing hypothesis. In A. Diamond (Ed.), The developmental and neural bases of higher cognitive functions (Annals of the New York Academy Sciences, Vol. 608, pp. 179–211). New York: New York Academy of Sciences.
Keele, S. W., Pokorny, R. A., Corcos, D. M., & Ivry, R. (1985). Do perception and motor production share common timing mech-anisms: A correlational analysis. Acta Psychologica, 60, 173–191. doi:10.1016/0001-6918(85)90054-X
Kellaris, J. J., & Kent, R. J. (1992). The influence of music on consumers’ temporal perceptions: Does time fly when you’re having fun? Journal of Consumer Psychology, 1, 365–376. doi:10.1016/S1057-7408(08)80060-5
Killeen, P. R., & Fetterman, J. G. (1988). A behavioral theory of timing. Psychological Review, 95, 274–295. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.95.2.274
Killeen, P. R., Fetterman, J. G., & Bizo, L. A. (1997). Time’s cause. In C. M. Bradshaw & E. Szabadi (Eds.), Time and behavior: Psychological and neurobehavioral analyses (pp. 79–131). Amsterdam: Elsevier, North-Holland.
Killeen, P. R., & Taylor, T. J. (2000). How the propagation of error through stochastic counters affects time discrimination and other psychophysical judgments. Psychological Review, 107, 430–459.
Killeen, P. R., & Weiss, N. A. (1987). Optimal timing and the Weber function. Psychological Review, 94, 455–468. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.107.3.430
Klapproth, F. (2009). Single-modality memory mixing in temporal generalization: An effect due to instructional ambiguity. Neuro-Quantology, 7, 85–94.
Koch, G., Oliveri, M., Carlesimo, G. A., & Caltagirone, C. (2002). Selective deficit of time perception in a patient with right prefrontal cortex lesion. Neurology, 59, 1658–1659.
Koch, G., Oliveri, M., Torriero, S., & Caltagirone, C. (2003). Underestimation of time perception after repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neurology, 60, 1844–1846.
Koch, G., Oliveri, M., Torriero, S., Salerno, S., Lo Gerfo, E. & Caltagirone, C. (2007). Repetitive TMS of cerebellum interferes with millisecond time processing. Experimental Brain Research, 179, 291–299.
Kudo, K., Miyazaki, M., Kimura, T., Yamanaka, K., Kadota, H., Hirashima, M., et al. (2004). Selective activation and deactivation of the human brain structures between speeded and precisely timed tapping responses to identical visual stimulus: An fMRI study. Neuro-Image, 22, 1291–1301.
Labelle, M.-A., Graf, P., Grondin, S., & Gagné-Roy, L. (2009). Time-related processes in time-based prospective memory and in time-interval production. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 21, 501–521. doi:10.1080/09541440802031000
Lapid, E., Ulrich, R., & Rammsayer, T. (2008). On estimating the difference limen in duration discrimination tasks: A comparison of the 2AFC and the reminder tasks. Perception & Psychophysics, 70, 291–305. doi:10.3758/PP.70.2.291
Lapid, E., Ulrich, R., & Rammsayer, T. (2009). Perceptual learning in auditory temporal discrimination: No evidence for a cross-modal transfer to the visual modality. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 382–389.
Large, E. W. (2008). Resonating to musical rhythm: Theory and experiment. In S. Grondin (Ed.), Psychology of time (pp. 189–232). Bingley, U.K.: Emerald Group.
Large, E. W., & Jones, M. R. (1999). The dynamics of attending: How we track time varying events. Psychological Review, 106, 119–159.
Lavoie, P., & Grondin, S. (2004). Information processing limitations as revealed by temporal discrimination. Brain & Cognition, 54, 198–200. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2004.02.039
Lee, K. H., Bhaker, R. S., Mysore, A., Parks, R. W., Birkett, P. B., & Woodruff, P. W. (2009). Time perception and its neuropsychological correlates in patients with schizophrenia and in healthy volunteers. Psychiatry Research, 166, 174–183.
Lee, K. H., Eagleston, P. N., Brown, W. H., Gregory, A.N., Barker, A. T., & Woodruff, P. W. R. (2007). The role of the cerebellum in subsecond time perception: Evidence from repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 147–157. doi:10.1162/jocn.2007.19.1.147
Lejeune, H., & Wearden, J. H. (2009). Vierordt’s The experimental study of the time sense (1868) and its legacy. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 21, 941–960.
Lewis, P. A., & Miall, R. C. (2003a). Brain activation patterns during measurement of sub- and supra-second intervals. Neuropsychologia, 41, 1583–1592. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(03)00118-0
Lewis, P. A., & Miall, R. C. (2003b). Distinct systems for automatic and cognitively controlled time measurement: Evidence from neuroimaging. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 13, 250–255. doi:10.1016/S0959-4388(03)00036-9
Lewis, P. A., & Miall, R. C. (2006). A right hemispheric prefrontal system for cognitive time measurement. Behavioural Processes, 71, 226–234. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2005.12.009
Lewis, P. A., & Miall, R. C. (2009). The precision of temporal judgement: Milliseconds, many minutes, and beyond. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 364, 1897–1905.
Lhamon, W. T., & Goldstone, S. (1974). Studies on auditory-visual differences in human time judgment: II. More transmitted information with sounds than lights. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 39, 295–307.
Livesey, A. C., Wall, M. B., & Smith, A. T. (2007). Time perception: Manipulation of task difficulty dissociates clock functions from other cognitive demands. Neuropsychologia, 45, 321–331. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.06.033
Lobo, F. S. N. (2008). Nature of time and causality in physics. In S. Grondin (Ed.), Psychology of time (pp. 395–422). Bingley, U.K.: Emerald Group.
Macar, F., Anton, J.-L., Bonnet, M., & Vidal, F. (2004). Timing functions of the supplementary motor area: An event-related fMRI study. Cognitive Brain Research, 21, 206–215. doi:10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.01.005
Macar, F., Coull, J., & Vidal, F. (2006). The supplementary motor area in motor and perceptual time processing: fMRI studies. Cognitive Processing, 7, 89–94. doi:10.1007/s10339-005-0025-7
Macar, F., Grondin, S., & Casini, L. (1994). Controlled attention sharing influences time estimation. Memory & Cognition, 22, 673–686.
Macar, F., Lejeune, H., Bonnet, M., Ferrara, A., Pouthas, V., Vidal, F., & Maquet, P. (2002). Activation of the supplementary motor area and of attentional networks during temporal processing. Experimental Brain Research, 142, 475–485.
Macar, F., & Vidal, F. (2004). Event-related potentials as indices of time processing: A review. Journal of Psychophysiology, 18, 89–104. doi:10.1027/0269-8803.18.23.89
Macar, F., & Vidal, F. (2009). Timing processes: An outline of behavioural and neural indices not systematically considered in timing models. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 227–239. doi:10.1037/a0014457
Macar, F., Vidal, F., & Casini, L. (1999). The supplementary motor area in motor and sensory timing: Evidence from slow brain potential changes. Experimental Brain Research, 135, 271–280.
Macmillan, N. A., & Creelman, C. D. (1991). Detection theory: A user’s guide. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Madison, G. (2001). Variability in isochronous tapping: Higher order dependencies as a function of intertap interval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 27, 411–422. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.27.2.411
Marzi, C. A. (2004). Two brains, one clock. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 1–3. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2003.10.015
Matell, M. S., & Meck, W. H. (2004). Cortico-striatal circuits and interval timing: Coincidence detection of oscillatory processes. Cognitive Brain Research, 21, 139–170. doi:10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.06.012
Matell, M. S., Meck, W. H., & Nicolelis, M. A. L. (2003). Integration of behavior and timing: Anatomically separate system or distributed processing? In W. H. Meck (Ed.), Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing (pp. 485–514). Boca Raton, FL: CRC.
Mauk, M. D., & Buonomano, D. V. (2004). The neural basis of temporal processing. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 307–340. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144247
McAuley, J. D., & Jones, M. R. (2003). Modeling effects of rhythmic context on perceived duration: A comparison of interval and entrainment approaches to short-interval timing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 29, 1102–1125. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.29.6.1102
McAuley, J. D., Jones, M. R., Holub, S., Johnston, H. M., & Miller, N. S. (2006). The time of our lives: Life span development of timing and event tracking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135, 348–367. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.135.3.348
McAuley, J. D., & Miller, N. S. (2007). Picking up the pace: Effects of global temporal context on sensitivity to the tempo of auditory sequences. Perception & Psychophysics, 69, 709–718.
McCormack, T., Wearden, J., Smith, M., & Brown, G. (2005). Epi sodic temporal generalization: A developmental study. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58A, 693–704. doi:10.1080/02724980443000250
Meck, W. H. (1984). Attentional bias between modalities: Effect on the internal clock, memory, and decision stages used in animal time discrimination. In J. Gibbon & L. G. Allan (Eds.), Timing and time perception (Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, Vol. 423, pp. 528–541). New York: New York Academy of Sciences.
Meck, W. H. (ED.) (2003). Functional and neural mechanisms of internal timing. Boca Raton, FL: CRC.
Meck, W. H. (ED.) (2004). Neuroimaging of interval timing [Special issue]. Cognitive Brain Research, 21(2). doi:10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.07.010
Meck, W. H. (ED.) (2005). Neuropsychology of timing and time perception [Special issue]. Brain & Cognition, 58(1). doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2004.09.004
Meck, W. H., & Benson, A. M. (2002). Dissecting the brain’s internal clock: How frontal-striatal circuitry keeps time and shifts attention. Brain & Cognition, 48, 195–211. doi:10.1006/brcg.2001.1313
Miller, N., & McAuley, J. D. (2005). Tempo sensitivity in isochronous tone sequences: The multiple-look model revisited. Perception & Psychophysics, 67, 1150–1160.
Mitsudo, T., Nakajima, Y., Remijn, G. B., Takeichi, H., Goto, Y., & Tobimatsu, S. (2009). Electrophysiological evidence of auditory temporal perception related to the assimilation between two neighboring time intervals. NeuroQuantology, 7, 114–127.
Monfort, V., & Pouthas, V. (2003). Effects of working memory demands on frontal slow waves in time-interval reproduction tasks in humans. Neuroscience Letters, 343, 195–199.
Morrone, M. C., Ross, J., & Burr, D. (2005). Saccadic eye movements cause compression of time as well as space. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 950–954. doi:10.1038/nn1488
Nakajima, Y., ten Hoopen, G., Sasaki, T., Yamamoto, K., Kado ta, M., Simons, M., & Suetomi, D. (2004). Time-shrinking: The process of unilateral temporal assimilation. Perception, 33, 1061–1079. doi:10.1068/p5061
N’Diaye, K., Ragot, R., Garnero, L., & Pouthas, V. (2004). What is common to brain activity evoked by the perception of visual and auditory filled durations? A study with MEG and EEG corecordings. Cognitive Brain Research, 21, 250–268. doi:10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.04.006
Nenadic, I., Gaser, C., Volz, H. P., Rammsayer, T., Hager, F., & Sauer, H. (2003). Processing of temporal information and the basal ganglia: New evidence from fMRI. Experimental Brain Research, 148, 238–246.
Nichelli, P., Always, D., & Grafman, J. (1996). Perceptual timing in cerebellar degeneration. Neuropsychologia, 34, 863–871. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(96)00001-2
Ogden, R. S., Wearden, J. H., & Jones, L. A. (2008). Double standards: Memory loading in temporal reference memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 34, 1524–1544. doi:10.1080/02724990344000088
Ono, F., & Kitazawa, S. (2010). Shortening of subjective tone intervals followed by repetitive tone stimuli. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 492–500.
Ornstein, R. (1969). On the experience of time. New York: Penguin.
Pariyadath, V., & Eagleman, D. (2007). The effect of predictability on subjective duration. PLoS ONE, 11, e1264. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0001264
Penney, T. B., Gibbon, J., & Meck, W. H. (2000). Differential effects of auditory and visual signals on clock speed and temporal memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 26, 1770–1787. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.26.6.1770
Penney, T. B., Gibbon, J., & Meck, W. H. (2008). Categorical scaling of duration bisection in pigeons (Columba livia), mice (Mus musculus), and humans (Homo sapiens). Psychological Science, 19, 1103–1109. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02210.x
Penney, T. B., & Vaitilingam, L. (2008). Imaging time. In S. Grondin (Ed.), Psychology of time (pp. 261–294). Bingley, U.K.: Emerald Group.
Perret-Clermont, A.-N. (ED.) (2005). Thinking time. Göttingen: Hogrefe & Huber.
Pfeuty, M., Ragot, R., & Pouthas, V. (2003a). Processes involved in tempo perception: A CNV analysis. Psychophysiology, 40, 69–76. doi:10.1111/1469-8986.00008
Pfeuty, M., Ragot, R., & Pouthas, V. (2003b). When time is up: CNV time course differentiates the roles of the hemispheres in the discrimination of short tone durations. Experimental Brain Research, 151, 372–379.
Pfeuty, M., Ragot, R., & Pouthas, V. (2008). Brain activity during interval timing depends on sensory structure. Brain Research, 1024, 112–117. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2008.01.022
Phillips, D. P., & Hall, S. E. (2002). Auditory temporal gap detection for noise markers with partially overlapping and non-overlapping spectra. Hearing Research, 174, 133–141.
Pöppel, E. (1997). A hierarchical model of temporal perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1, 56–61. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(97)01008-5
Pöppel, E. (2004). Lost in time: A historical frame, elementary processing units and the 3-second window. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis, 64, 295–301.
Pouthas, V., Garnero, L., Ferrandez, A.-M., & Renault, B. (2000). ERPs and PET analysis of time perception: Spatial and temporal brain mapping during visual discrimination tasks. Human Brain Mapping, 10, 49–60. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(200006)10:2<49::AID-HBM10>3.0.CO;2-8
Pouthas, V., George, N., Poline, J.-B., Pfeuty, M., VandeMoorteele, P.-F., Hugueville, L., et al. (2005). Neural network involved in time perception: An fMRI study comparing long and short interval estimation. Human Brain Mapping, 25, 433–441. doi:10.1002/hbm.20126
Predebon, J. (1996). The effects of active and passive processing of interval events on prospective and retrospective time estimates. Acta Psychologica, 94, 41–58. doi:10.1016/0001-6918(95)00044-5
Quené, H. (2007). On the just noticeable difference for tempo in speech. Journal of Phonetics, 35, 353–362. doi:10.1016/j.wocn.2006.09.001
Rammsayer, T. H. (2008). Neuropharmacological approaches to human timing. In S. Grondin (Ed.), Psychology of time (pp. 295–320). Bingley, U.K.: Emerald Group.
Rammsayer, T. H., & Lima, S. D. (1991). Duration discrimination of filled and empty auditory intervals: Cognitive and perceptual factors. Perception & Psychophysics, 50, 565–574.
Rammsayer, T. [H.], & Ulrich, R. (2001). Counting models of temporal discrimination. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 270–277.
Rammsayer, T. [H.], & Ulrich, R. (2005). No evidence for qualitative difference in the processing of short and long temporal intervals. Acta Psychologica, 120, 141–171.
Rao, S. M., Mayer, A. R., & Harrington, D. L. (2001). The evolution of brain activation during temporal processing. Nature Neuroscience, 4, 317–323. doi:10.1038/85191
Rattat, A.-C., & Droit-Volet, S. (2005). The long-term retention of time: A developmental study. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58B, 163–176.
Rattat, A.-C., & Droit-Volet, S. (2007). Implicit long-term memory for duration in young children. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 19, 271–285. doi:10.1080/09541440600834647
Rau, P.-L. P., Shu-Yun, P., & Chin-Chow, Y. (2006). Time distortion for expert and novice online game players. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 9, 396–403. doi:10.1089/cpb.2006.9.396
Repp, B. H. (2005). Sensorimotor synchronization: A review of the tapping literature. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 969–992.
Roberts, W. A. (2008). Can animals cognitively travel to the past and future? In S. Grondin (Ed.), Psychology of time (pp. 322–344). Bingley, U.K.: Emerald Group.
Robertson, S., Zelaznik, H., Lantero, D., Gadacz, K., Spencer, R., Doffin, J., & Schneidt, T. (1999). Correlations for timing consistency among tapping and drawing tasks: Evidence against a single timing process for motor control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 25, 1316–1330. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.25.5.1316
Roeckelein, J. E. (2000). The concept of time in psychology: A resource book and annotated bibliography. Westport, CT: Greenwood.
Roeckelein, J. E. (2008). History of conceptions and accounts of time and early time perception research. In S. Grondin (Ed.), Psychology of time (pp. 1–50). Bingley, U.K.: Emerald Group.
Rosenbaum, D. A. (2002). Time, space, and short-term memory. Brain & Cognition, 48, 52–65. doi:10.1006/brcg.2001.1303
Roussel, M.-E., Grondin, S., & Killeen, P. (2009). Spatial effects on temporal categorization. Perception, 38, 748–762.
Roy, M. M., & Christenfeld, N. J. S. (2008). Effect of task length on remembered and predicted duration. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 202–207. doi:10.3758/PBR.15.1.202
Roy, M. M., Christenfeld, N. J. S., & McKenzie, C. R. M. (2005). Underestimation of future duration: Memory incorrectly used or memory bias? Psychological Bulletin, 131, 738–756. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.131.5.738
Rubia, K. (2006). The neural corrolates of timing functions. In J. Glicksohn & M. S. Myslobodsky (Eds.), Timing the future: The case for a time-based prospective memory (pp. 213–238). River Edge, NJ: World Scientific Publishing.
Sarrazin, J.-C., Giraudo, M.-D., Pailhous, J., & Bootsma, R. J. (2004). Dynamics of balancing space and time in memory: Tau and kappa effects revisited. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 30, 411–430. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.30.3.411
Sarrazin, J.-C., Giraudo, M.-D., & Pittenger, J. B. (2007). Tau and kappa effects in physical space: The case of audition. Psychological Research, 71, 201–218. doi:10.1007/s00426-005-0019-1
Schöner, G. (2002). Timing, clocks, and dynamical systems. Brain & Cognition, 48, 31–51. doi:10.1006/brcg.2001.1302
Smith, A., Taylor, E., Lidzba, K., & Rubia, K. (2003). A right hemispheric frontocerebellar network for time discrimination of several hundreds of milliseconds. NeuroImage, 20, 344–350.
Smith, J. G., Harper, D. N., Gittings, D., & Abernethy, D. (2007). The effect of Parkinson’s disease on time estimation as a function of stimulus duration range and modality. Brain & Cognition, 64, 130–143. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2007.01.005
Spencer, R. M. C., & Zelaznik, H. N. (2003). Weber (slope) analyses of timing variability in tapping and drawing tasks. Journal of Motor Behavior, 35, 371–382.
Spencer, R. M. C., Zelaznik, H. N., Diedrichsen, J., & Ivry, R. B. (2003). Disrupted timing of discontinuous but not continuous movements by cerebellar lesions. Science, 300, 1437–1439.
Staddon, J. E. R., & Higa, J. J. (1996). Multiple time scales in simple habituation. Psychological Review, 103, 720–733. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.103.4.720
Staddon, J. E. R., & Higa, J. J. (1999). Time and memory: Towards a pacemaker-free theory of interval timing. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 71, 215–251. doi:10.1901/jeab.1999.71-215
Strathman, A., & Joireman, J. (Eds.) (2005). Understanding behavior in the context of time. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Szelag, E., & Wittmann, M. (EDS.) (2004). Time, cognition, thinking [Special issue]. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis, 64(3).
Tallal, P. (2003). Language learning disabilities: Integrating research approaches. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 206–211. doi:10.1046/j.0963-7214.2003.01263.x
Tallal, P. (2004). Improving language and literacy is a matter of time. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 721–728. doi:10.1038/nrn1499
Tarlaci, S. (ED.) (2009). Time, timing, and the brain [Special issue]. NeuroQuantology, 7.
ten Hoopen, G., Miyauchi, R., & Nakajima, Y. (2008). Time-based illusions in the auditory mode. In S. Grondin (Ed.), Psychology of time (pp. 139–188). Bingley, U.K.: Emerald Group.
Thaut, M. H. (2005). Rhythm, music and the brain: Scientific foundations and clinical applications. New York: Taylor & Francis.
Thomas, K. E., Handley, S. J., & Newstead, S. E. (2007). The role of prior task experience in temporal misestimating. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 230–240.
Tobin, S., Bisson, N., & Grondin, S. (2010). An ecological approach to prospective and retrospective timing of long durations: A study involving gamers. PLoS ONE, 5(2), e9271. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0009271
Tobin, S., & Grondin, S. (2009). Video games and the perception of very long durations by adolescents. Computers in Human Behavior, 25, 554–559. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2008.12.002
Toplak, M. E., Dockstader, C., & Tannock, R. (2006). Temporal information processing in ADHD: Findings to date and new methods. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 151, 15–26.
Tracy, J. I., Faro, S. H., Mohamed, F. B., Pinsk, M., & Pinus, A. (2000). Functional localization of a “time keeper” function separate from attentional resources and task strategy. NeuroImage, 11, 228–242.
Tregellas, J. R., Davalos, D. B., & Rojas, D. C. (2006). Effect of task difficulty on the functional anatomy of temporal processing. Neuro-Image, 32, 307–315.
Treisman, M. (1963). Temporal discrimination and the indifference interval: Implications for a model of the “internal clock.” Psychological Monographs, 77(Whole no. 576).
Treisman, M., Faulkner, A., Naish, P. L. N., & Brogan, D. (1990). The internal clock: Evidence for a temporal oscillation underlying time perception with some estimates of its characteristic frequency. Perception, 19, 705–743.
Tse, C.-Y., & Penney, T. B. (2006). Preattentive timing of empty intervals is from marker offset to onset. Psychophysiology, 43, 172–179. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2006.389.x
Tse, P. U., Intriligator, J., Rivest, J., & Cavanagh, P. (2004). Attention and the subjective expansion of time. Perception & Psychophysics, 66, 1171–1189.
Tulving, E. (2002). Chronesthesia: Conscious awareness of subjective time. In D. T. Stuss & R. T. Knight (Eds.), Principles of frontal lobe function (pp. 311–325). New York: Oxford University Press.
Ulrich, R., Nitschke, J., & Rammsayer, T. (2006). Crossmodal temporal discrimination: Assessing the predictions of a general pacemaker- counter model. Perception & Psychophysics, 68, 1140–1152.
Vicario, G. B. (2005). Il tempo. Saggio di psicologia sperimentale [On time: An essay in experimental psychology]. Bologna, Italy: Il Mulino.
Volz, H.-P., Nenadic, I., Gaser, C., Rammsayer, T., Hager, F., & Sauer, H. (2001). Time estimation in schizophrenia: An fMRI study at adjusted levels of difficulty. NeuroReport, 12, 313–316. doi:10.1097/00001756-200102120-00026
Wackermann, J. (2007). Inner and outer horizons of time experience. Spanish Journal of Psychology, 10, 20–32.
Wackermann, J., & Ehm, W. (2006). The dual klepsydra model of internal time representation and time reproduction. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 239, 482–493. doi:10.1016/j.jtbi.2005.08.024
Walker, J. T., & Scott, K. J. (1981). Auditory-visual conflicts in the perceived duration of lights, tones, and gaps. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 7, 1327–1339. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.7.6.1327
Wallace, M., & Rabin, A. I. (1960). Temporal experience. Psychological Bulletin, 57, 213–235. doi:10.1037/h0041410
Wearden, J. H. (1992). Temporal generalization in humans. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 18, 134–144. doi:10.1037/0097-7403.18.2.134
Wearden, J. H. (2003). Applying the scalar timing model to human time psychology: Progress and challenges. In H. Helfrich (Ed.), Time and mind II (pp. 21–39). Göttingen: Hogrefe & Huber.
Wearden, J. H. (2004). Decision processes in models of timing. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis, 64, 303–317.
Wearden, J. H., Edwards, H., Fakhri, M., & Percival, A. (1998). Why “sounds are judged longer than lights”: Application of a model of the internal clock in humans. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51B, 97–120.
Wearden, J. H., & Lejeune, H. (2008). Scalar properties in human timing: Conformity and violations. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 569–587. doi:10.1080/17470210701282576
Wearden, J. H., Norton, R., Martin, S., & Montford-Bebb, O. (2007). Internal clock processes and the filled-duration illusion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 33, 716–729. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.33.3.716
Wearden, J. H., Smith-Spark, J. H., Cousins, R., Edelstyn, N. M. J., Cody, F. W. J., O’Boyle, D. J. (2008). Stimulus timing by people with Parkinson’s disease. Brain & Cognition, 67, 264–279. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2008.01.010
Wing, A. M. (2002). Voluntary timing and brain function: An information processing approach. Brain & Cognition, 48, 7–30. doi:10.1006/brcg.2001.1301
Wing, A. M., & Kristofferson, A. B. (1973). Response delays and the timing of discrete motor responses. Perception & Psychophysics, 14, 5–12.
Wittmann, M., & van Wassenhove, V. (EDS.) (2009). The experience of time: Neural mechanisms and the interplay of emotion, cognition and embodiment [Special issue]. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 364(1525). doi:10.1098/rstb.2009.0025
Woodrow, H. (1934). The temporal indifference interval determined by the method of average error. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 17, 167–188.
Yarrow, K., Haggard, P., Heal, R., Brown, P., & Rothwell, J. C. (2001). Illusory perceptions of space and time preserve cross-saccadic perceptual continuity. Nature, 414, 302–305. doi:10.1038/35104551
Yarrow, K., & Rothwell, J. C. E. (2003). Manual chronostasis: Tactile perception precedes physical contact. Current Biology, 13, 1334–1339. doi:10.1016/S0960-9822(03)00413-5
Zakay, D. (1993). Time estimation methods—Do they influence prospective duration estimates? Perception, 22, 91–101. doi:10.1068/p220091
Zakay, D. (1998). Attention allocation policy influences prospective timing. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5, 114–118.
Zakay, D., & Block, R. A. (1997). Temporal cognition. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 6, 12–16. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.ep11512604
Zakay, D., & Block, R. A. (2004). Prospective and retrospective duration judgments: An executive-control perspective. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis, 64, 319–328.
Zeiler, M. D. (1998). On sundials, springs, and atoms. Behavioural Processes, 44, 89–99. doi:10.1016/S0376-6357(98)00042-4
Zeiler, M. D. (1999). Time without clocks. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 71, 288–291. doi:10.1901/jeab.1999.71-288
Zelaznik, H. N., Spencer, R. M. [C.], & Doffin, J. G. (2000). Temporal precision in tapping and circle drawing movements at preferred rates is not correlated: Further evidence against timing as a general purpose ability. Journal of Motor Behavior, 32, 193–199.
Zelaznik, H. N., Spencer, R. M. C., & Ivry, R. B. (2002). Dissociation of explicit and implicit timing in repetitive tapping and drawing movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 28, 575–588. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.28.3.575
Zelaznik, H. N., Spencer, R. M. C., & Ivry, R. B. (2008). Behavioral analysis of human movement timing. In S. Grondin (Ed.), Psychology of time (pp. 233–260). Bingley, U.K.: Emerald Group.