Theory of Event Coding (TEC) V2.0: Representing and controlling perception and action

Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics - Tập 81 - Trang 2139-2154 - 2019
Bernhard Hommel1
1Institute of Psychology, Cognitive Psychology Unit, University of Leiden, Leiden, The Netherlands

Tóm tắt

This article provides an update of the Theory of Event Coding (TEC), which claims that perception and action are identical processes operating on the same codes – event files consisting of integrated networks of sensorimotor feature codes. The original version of the theory emphasized its representational underpinnings, but recent theoretical developments provide the basis for a more integrated view consisting of both the codes that are shared between perception and action in the control processes operating on these codes. Four developments are discussed in more detail: The degree to which the integration and retrieval of event files depends on current goals, how metacontrol states impact the handling of event files, how feature binding relates to event learning, and how the integration of non-social events relates to the integration of social events. Case examples using various versions of the Simon task are used to explain how the new version of TEC explains interactions between perception and action in non-social and social situations.

Tài liệu tham khảo

Ach, N. (1910). Über den Willensakt und das Temperament. Leipzig: Quelle & Meyer. Ach, N. (1935). Analyse des Willens. In E. Abderhalden (Ed.), Handbuch der biologischen Arbeitsmethoden (Vol. VI). Berlin: Urban & Schwarzenberg. Berlyne, D.E. (1949). ‘Interest’ as a psychological concept. British Journal of Psychology, 39, 184-195. Berlyne, D.E. (1960). Conflict, Arousal and Curiosity. New York: McGraw Hill. Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Wohlschläger, A., & Prinz, W. (2000). Compatibility between observed and executed finger movements: Comparing symbolic, spatial, and imitative cues. Brain and Cognition, 44, 124-143. Brunswik, E. (1944). Distal focussing of perception. Size constancy in a representative sample of situations. Psychological Monographs, 56, No. 1. Colzato, L.S., van Wouwe, N.C., Lavender, T.J., & Hommel, B. (2006a). Intelligence and cognitive flexibility: Fluid intelligence correlates with feature "unbinding" across perception and action. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 1043-1048. Colzato, L. S., Raffone, A., & Hommel, B. (2006b). What do we learn from binding features? Evidence for multilevel feature integration. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 705-716. Colzato, L.S., Zech, H., Hommel, B., Verdonschot, R., van den Wildenberg, W., & Hsieh, S. (2012a). Loving-kindness brings loving-kindness: The impact of Buddhism on cognitive self-other integration. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 541-545. Colzato, L.S., de Bruijn, E., & Hommel, B. (2012b). Up to "me" or up to "us"? The impact of self-construal priming on cognitive self-other integration. Frontiers in Psychology, 3:341. Cools, R., & D’Esposito M (2010). Dopaminergic modulation of flexible cognitive control in humans. In: A. Björklund, S. Dunnett, L. Iversen, & S. Iversen (eds.), Dopamine handbook (pp. 249-260). Oxford: Oxford University Press. DeYoe, E. A., & Van Essen, D. C. (1988). Concurrent processing streams in monkey visual cortex. Trends in Neuroscience, 11, 219-226. Dolk, T., Hommel, B., Colzato, L.S., Schütz-Bosbach, S., Prinz, W., & Liepelt, R. (2011). How 'social' is the social Simon effect? Frontiers in Psychology, 2:84. Dolk, T., Hommel, B., Prinz, W., & Liepelt, R. (2013). The (not so) Social Simon effect: A referential coding account. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39, 1248-1260. Dreisbach, G. & Goschke, T. (2004). How positive affect modulates cognitive control: Reduced perseveration at the cost of increased distractibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 30, 343-353. Durstewitz, D., & Seamans, J.K. (2008). The dual-state theory of prefrontal cortex dopamine function with relevance to catechol-o-methyltransferase genotypes and schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 64, 739-749. Elsner, B., & Hommel, B. (2001). Effect anticipation and action control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 229-240. Elsner, B., Hommel, B., Mentschel, C., Drzezga, A., Prinz, W., Conrad, B., & Siebner, H. R. (2002). Linking actions and their perceivable consequences in the human brain. Neuroimage, 17, 364-372. Fagioli, S., Hommel, B., & Schubotz, R.I. (2007). Intentional control of attention: Action planning primes action-related stimulus dimensions. Psychological Research, 71, 22-29. Feldman Barrett, L. (2017). How emotions are made: The secret life of the brain. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt. Frings, C., Merz, S., & Hommel, B. (2019). The impact of stimulus uncertainty on attentional control. Cognition, 183, 208-212. Georgopoulos, A. P. (1990). Neurophysiology of reaching. In M. Jeannerod (Ed.), Attention and Performance XIII: Motor representation and control (pp. 227-263). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Goschke, T. (2003). Voluntary action and cognitive control from a cognitive neuroscience perspective. In S. Maasen, W. Prinz, & G. Roth (Hrsg.), Voluntary action: Brains, minds, and sociality (49-85). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Greenwald, A.G., Banaji, M.R., Rudman, L.A., Farnham, S.D., Nosek, B.A., & Mellott, D.S. (2002). A unified theory of implicit attitudes, stereotypes, self-esteem, and self-concept. Psychological Review, 109, 3–25. Haazebroek, P., Raffone, A., & Hommel, B. (2017). HiTEC: A connectionist model of the interaction between perception and action planning. Psychological Research, 81, 1085-1109. Harvey, P. (2012). An introduction to Buddhism: Teachings, history and practices. Cambridge: University Press. Heider, F. (1926/1959). Thing and medium. Psychological Issues, 1959, Monograph 3 (original work published 1926). Heister, G., Ehrenstein, W.H., & Schroder-Heister, P. (1986). Spatial S-R compatibility effects with unimanual two-finger choice reactions for prone and supine hand positions. Perception & Psychophysics, 40, 271-227. Hommel, B. (1993). Inverting the Simon effect by intention: Determinants of direction and extent of effects of irrelevant spatial information. Psychological Research, 55, 270-279. Hommel, B. (1995). S-R compatibility and the Simon effect: Toward an empirical clarification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 764-775. Hommel, B. (1996). S-R compatibility effects without response uncertainty. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A, 546-571. Hommel, B. (1998). Event files: Evidence for automatic integration of stimulus-response episodes. Visual Cognition, 5, 183-216. Hommel, B. (2000). Intentional control of automatic stimulus-response translation. In Y. Rossetti & A. Revonsuo (eds.), Interaction between dissociable conscious and nonconscious processes (pp. 223-244). Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company. Hommel, B. (2004). Event files: Feature binding in and across perception and action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 494-500. Hommel, B. (2009). Action control according to TEC (theory of event coding). Psychological Research, 73, 512-526. Hommel, B. (2011). The Simon effect as tool and heuristic. Acta Psychologica, 136, 189-202. Hommel, B. (2015a). The theory of event coding (TEC) as embodied-cognition framework. Frontiers in Psychology, 6:1318. Hommel, B. (2015b). Between persistence and flexibility: The Yin and Yang of action control. In: A.J. Elliot (ed.), Advances in motivation science, Vol. 2 (pp. 33-67). New York: Elsevier. Hommel, B. (2016). Embodied cognition according to TEC. In: Y. Coello & M. Fischer (eds.), Foundations of embodied cognition, Volume 1: Perceptual and emotional embodiment (pp. 75-92). Psychology Press. Hommel, B. (2018). Representing oneself and others: An event-coding approach. Experimental Psychology, 65, 323-331. Hommel, B., & Colzato, L.S. (2009). When an object is more than a binding of its features: Evidence for two mechanisms of visual feature integration. Visual Cognition, 17, 120-140. Hommel, B., & Colzato, L.S. (2015). Interpersonal trust: An event-based account. Frontiers in Psychology, 6:1399. Hommel, B., & Colzato, L.S. (2017a). The social transmission of metacontrol policies: Mechanisms underlying the interpersonal transfer of persistence and flexibility. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 81, 43-58. Hommel, B., & Colzato, L.S. (2017b). Meditation and metacontrol. Journal of Cognitive Enhancement, 1, 115-121. Hommel, B., & Wiers, R.W. (2017). Towards a unitary approach to human action control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21, 940-949. Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001a). The Theory of Event Coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849-937. Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001b). Codes and their vicissitudes. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 910-937. Hommel, B., Proctor, R.W., & Vu, K.-P.L. (2004). A feature-integration account of sequential effects in the Simon task. Psychological Research, 68, 1-17. Hommel, B., Kray, J., & Lindenberger, U. (2011). Feature integration across the lifespan: Stickier stimulus-response bindings in children and older adults. Frontiers in Psychology, 2:268. James, W. (1884). What is an emotion? Mind, 9, 188–205. Kachergis, G., Wyatte, D., O'Reilly, R.C., de Kleijn, R., & Hommel, B. (2014). A continuous time neural model for sequential action. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 369, 20130623. Kahnemann, D., Treisman, A., & Gibbs, B.J. (1992). The reviewing of object files: Object-specific integration of information. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 175–219. Kalaska, J. F., & Hyde, M. L. (1985). Area 4 and area 5: Differences between the load direction-dependent discharge variability of cells during active postural fixation. Experimental Brain Research, 59, 197-202. Keizer, A.W., Verschoor, M., Verment, R., & Hommel, B. (2010a). The effect of gamma enhancing neurofeedback on measures of feature-binding flexibility and intelligence. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 75, 25-32. Keizer, A.W., Verment, R., & Hommel, B. (2010b). Enhancing cognitive control through neurofeedback: A role of gamma-band activity in managing episodic retrieval. Neuroimage, 49, 3404-3413. Kilner, J., Hommel, B., Bar, M., Barsalou, L.W., Friston, K.J., Jost, J., Maye, A., Metzinger, T., Pulvermüller, F., Sánchez-Fibla, M., Tsotsos, J.K., & Vigliocco, G. (2015). Action-oriented models of cognitive processing: A little less cogitation, a little more action please. In: A.K. Engel, K.J. Friston & D. Kragic (eds.), The pragmatic turn: Toward action-oriented views in cognitive science, (pp. 159-172). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Kim, D., & Hommel, B. (2015). An event-based account of conformity. Psychological Science, 26, 484-489. Knoblich, G., & Sebanz, N. (2006). The social nature of perception and action. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 99–104. Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis of stimulus-response compatibility—A model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97, 253-170. Kühn, S., Keizer, A., Colzato, L.S., Rombouts, S.A.R.B., & Hommel, B. (2011a). The neural underpinnings of event-file management: Evidence for stimulus-induced activation of, and competition among stimulus-response bindings. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 896-904. Kühn, S., Keizer, A., Rombouts, S.A.R.B., & Hommel, B. (2011b). The functional and neural mechanism of action preparation: Roles of EBA and FFA in voluntary action control. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 214-220. Kühnen, U., & Oyserman, D. (2002). Thinking about the self influences thinking in general: Cognitive consequences of salient self-concept. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 492–499. Logan, D.L. (1988). Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychological Review, 95, 492–527. Loveless, N.E. (1962). Direction-of-motion stereotypes: A review. Ergonomics, 5, 357-383. Lutz, A., Slagter, H. A., Dunne, J. D., & Davidson, R. J. (2008). Attention regulation and monitoring in meditation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 163–169. Mekern, V.N., Sjoerds, Z., & Hommel, B. (2019). How metacontrol biases and adaptivity impact performance in cognitive search tasks. Cognition, 182, 251-259. Memelink, J., & Hommel, B. (2013). Intentional weighting: A basic principle in cognitive control. Psychological Research, 77, 249-259. Müsseler, J., & Hommel, B. (1997). Blindness to response-compatible stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 861-872. O’Reilly, R.C., Bhattacharyya, R., Howard, M.D., & Ketz, N. (2014). Complementary learning systems. Cognitive Science, 38, 1229-1248. Pezzulo, G., Rigoli, F. & Friston, K. (2018). Hierarchical active inference: A theory of motivated control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22, 294-306. Prinz, W. (1992). Why don't we perceive our brain states? European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 4, 1-20. Saenz, M., & Langers, D.R.M. (2014). Tonotopic mapping of human auditory cortex. Hearing Research, 307, 42-52. Sebanz, N., Knoblich, G.., & Prinz, W. (2003). Representing others‘ actions: just like one’s own? Cognition 88, B11-B21. Shin, Y. K., Proctor, R. W., & Capaldi, E. J. (2010). A review of contemporary ideomotor theory. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 943–974. Simon, J. R., & Rudell, A. P. (1967). Auditory S-R compatibility: The effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. Journal of Applied Psychology, 51, 300-304. Sokolov, E.N. (1963). Perception and the conditioned reflex. Oxford: Pergamon Press. Spapé, M., & Hommel, B. (2008). He said, she said: Episodic retrieval induces conflict adaptation in an auditory Stroop task. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 1117-1121. Sternberg, S. (1969) The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donders' method. Acta Psychologica, 30, 276-315. Stock, A. & Stock, C. (2004). A short history of ideo-motor action. Psychological Research, 68, 176-188. Stoet, G., & Hommel, B. (1999). Action planning and the temporal binding of response codes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1625-1640. Stoet, G., & Hommel, B. (2002). Interaction between feature binding in perception and action. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Common mechanisms in perception and action: Attention & Performance XIX (pp. 538-552). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Treisman, A. (1996). The binding problem. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 6, 171-178. Verbruggen, F., McLaren, I.P.L., & Chambers, C.D. (2014). Banishing the control homunculi in studies of action control and behavior change. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9, 497-524. Verschoor, S.A., & Hommel, B. (2017). Self-by-doing: The role of action for self-acquisition. Social Cognition, 35, 127-145. Von der Malsburg, C. (1999). The what and why of binding: The modeler's perspective. Neuron, 24, 95-104. Wallace, R. A. (1971). S-R compatibility and the idea of a response code. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 88, 354-360. Yamaguchi, M., Wall, H.J., & Hommel, B. (2018). Sharing tasks or sharing actions? Evidence from the joint Simon task. Psychological Research, 82, 385-394. Zmigrod, S., de Sonneville, L.M.J., Colzato, L.S., Swaab, H. & Hommel, B. (2013). Cognitive control of feature bindings: Evidence from children with autistic spectrum disorder. Psychological Research, 77, 147-154.