The intelligent use of space
Tài liệu tham khảo
P.E. Agre, The Dynamics of Everyday Life (Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, England, to appear).
P.E. Agre, Hassles, MIT AI Lab Working Paper, Cambridge, MA.
Agre, 1987, Pengi: an implementation of a theory of activity
Agre, 1992, Cultural support for improvisation
Beach, 1985, Learning to become a bartender: the role of external memory cues at work
Brooks, 1991, Intelligence without representation, Artif. Intell., 47, 141, 10.1016/0004-3702(91)90053-M
Chambers, 1985, Can mental images be ambiguous?, J. Experimental Psychol. Human Perception Performance, 11, 317, 10.1037/0096-1523.11.3.317
Chapman, 1989, Penguins can make cake, AI Mag., 10, 45
Charness, 1981, Search in chess: age and skill differences, J. Experimental Psychol. Human Perception Performance, 7, 467, 10.1037/0096-1523.7.2.467
Chase, 1973, Perception in chess, Cogn. Psychol., 4, 55, 10.1016/0010-0285(73)90004-2
Cleveland, 1985, Graphical perception and graphical methods for analyzing scientific data, Science, 229, 828, 10.1126/science.229.4716.828
Cohen, 1989
de la Rocha, 1985, The reorganization of arithmetic practice in the kitchen, Anthropology Education Q., 16, 193, 10.1525/aeq.1985.16.3.05x1485c
Elster, 1982
Gibson, 1977, The theory of affordances
Goodnow, 1986, Some lifelong everyday forms of intelligent behavior: organizing and reorganizing, 143
Greeno, 1989, Situations, mental models, and generative knowledge, 285
Hammond, 1995, The stabilization of environments, Artif. Intell., 10.1016/0004-3702(94)00006-M
E. Hutchins, How a cockpit remembers its speeds, Cogn. Sci. (to appear).
E. Hutchins, Cognition in the Wild (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, to appear).
Kamman, 1982
Kirsh, 1991, When is information explicitly represented?, 340
Kirsh, 1991, Today the earwig, tomorrow man?, Artif. Intell., 47, 161, 10.1016/0004-3702(91)90054-N
Kirsh, 1992, Some epistemic benefits of action: Tetris, a case study
D. Kirsh and P. Maglio, On distinguishing epistemic from pragmatic actions, Cogn. Sci. (to appear).
Kotovsky, 1989, Representation and transfer in problem solving, 69
Lansdale, 1991, Remembering about documents: memory for appearance, format, and location, Ergonomics, 34, 1161, 10.1080/00140139108964853
Lansdale, 1992, Using memory for events in the design of personal filing systems, Int. J. Man-Mach. Stud., 97, 10.1016/0020-7373(92)90054-O
Larkin, 1989, Display-based problem solving, 319
Larkin, 1987, Why a diagram is (sometimes) worth ten thousand words, Cogn. Sci., 11, 65, 10.1111/j.1551-6708.1987.tb00863.x
Lave, 1977, Cognitive consequences of traditional apprenticeship training in West Africa, Anthropology Education Quart., 8, 177, 10.1525/aeq.1977.8.3.05x1512d
Lave, 1988
Malone, 1983, How do people organize their desks? Implications for the design of office automation systems, ACM Trans. Office Syst., 11, 99, 10.1145/357423.357430
Norman, 1981, Categorization of action slips, Psychol. Rev., 88, 1, 10.1037/0033-295X.88.1.1
Norman, 1988
Rasmussen, 1980, What can be learned from human error reports?
Rasmussen, 1986
Reason, 1990
Reason, 1982
Reisberg, 1987, External representations and the advantages of externalizing one's thoughts
Rouse, 1981, Models of human problem solving: detection, diagnosis and compensation for system failures
Savage, 1976
Scribner, 1986, Thinking in action: some characteristics of practical thought, 13
Simon, 1976, The understanding process: problem isomorphs, Cogn. Psychol., 8, 165, 10.1016/0010-0285(76)90022-0
Stevens, 1985, An observational study of skilled memory in waitresses
Tarjan, 1985, Amortized computational complexity, SIAM J. Comput., 14, 306