The inherence heuristic: An intuitive means of making sense of the world, and a potential precursor to psychological essentialism

Behavioral and Brain Sciences - Tập 37 Số 5 - Trang 461-480 - 2014
Andrei Cimpian1, Erika Salomon1
1Department of Psychology, University of Illinois, Champaign, IL

Tóm tắt

Abstract

We propose that human reasoning relies on aninherence heuristic, an implicit cognitive process that leads people to explain observed patterns (e.g., girls wear pink) predominantly in terms of the inherent features of their constituents (e.g., pink is a delicate color). We then demonstrate how this proposed heuristic can provide a unified account for a broad set of findings spanning areas of research that might at first appear unrelated (e.g., system justification, nominal realism, is–ought errors in moral reasoning). By revealing the deep commonalities among the diverse phenomena that fall under its scope, our account is able to generate new insights into these phenomena, as well as new empirical predictions. A second main goal of this article, aside from introducing the inherence heuristic, is to articulate the proposal that the heuristic serves as a foundation for the development of psychological essentialism. More specifically, we propose thatessentialism – which is the common belief that natural and social categories are underlain by hidden, causally powerful essences – emerges over the first few years of life as an elaboration of the earlier, and more open-ended, intuitions supplied by the inherence heuristic. In the final part of the report, we distinguish our proposal from competing accounts (e.g., Strevens's K-laws) and clarify the relationship between the inherence heuristic and related cognitive tendencies (e.g., the correspondence bias). In sum, this article illuminates a basic cognitive process that emerges early in life and is likely to have profound effects on many aspects of human psychology.

Từ khóa


Tài liệu tham khảo

10.1017/S0140525X00003435

10.1016/j.cogpsych.2005.05.003

10.1016/S0010-0277(01)00146-9

10.2307/2798500

10.1037/0033-2909.97.1.74

10.1037/0022-3514.43.5.889

Stanovich, 1999, Who is rational? Studies of individual differences in reasoning, 10.4324/9781410603432

Raven, 1960, Guide to the standard progressive matrices: Sets A, B, C, D and E. H. K.

10.2307/1131446

Bargh, 1999, Dual-process theories in social psychology, 361

10.2307/1130064

10.1016/0010-0285(75)90024-9

Shweder, 1987, The emergence of morality in young children, 1

10.1037/0022-3514.79.3.344

10.1177/0146167214541659

10.1037/0022-3514.50.4.703

10.4159/harvard.9780674433014

10.1007/BF00055564

10.1111/j.1750-8606.2010.00156.x

10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00504.x

10.1037/0033-2909.119.1.3

10.1037/e412952005-009

10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01865.x

McRae, 2013, The Oxford handbook of cognitive psychology

10.1016/j.tics.2012.06.004

Higgins, 1996, Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles, 133

10.1037/0012-1649.36.5.582

10.1017/CBO9780511808098.004

Trope, 1999, A dual-process model of overconfident attributional inferences, Dual-process theories in social psychology, 161

10.1037/0022-3514.36.11.1202

10.1080/01973533.2010.526877

10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00495.x

10.1162/POSC_a_00048

Markman, 1989, Categorization and naming in children: Problems of induction

10.1111/j.1530-2415.2006.00102.x

10.1037/0022-3514.67.6.949

10.1093/analys/60.1.13

10.1016/j.cognition.2007.10.006

10.1016/S0378-2166(99)00004-1

10.1111/j.1467-9221.2009.00733.x

10.1037/0022-3514.75.1.33

10.1073/pnas.1208951109

10.2307/2184373

10.1007/978-1-4899-0448-5

10.1016/S0010-0277(01)00145-7

10.3758/BF03196323

10.1177/0146167299258003

Premack, 1996, The cognitive neurosciences, 205

10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01192.x

10.1037/1089-2680.2.2.175

10.1037/h0034747

Kornblith, 1995, Inductive inference and its natural ground: An essay in naturalistic epistemology, 10.7551/mitpress/3730.001.0001

Laszlo, 2007, Citrus: A history

10.1521/soco.2011.29.3.231

10.1016/S0022-0965(02)00121-2

10.1037/0012-1649.35.3.835

10.1037/0022-3514.88.4.686

10.1023/A:1008290415597

10.1111/j.1551-6709.2009.01018.x

10.1111/j.1467-9221.2004.00402.x

10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00377.x

10.1016/j.jesp.2005.03.003

Dweck, 1999, Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality, and development

Ramachandran, 2001, Synaesthesia – A window into perception, thought and language, Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8, 3

10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00071-8

Kruglanski, 1996, Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles, 493

10.1177/0963721411422059

10.1016/j.cognition.2010.06.011

Atran, 1993, Cognitive foundations of natural history: Towards an anthropology of science

10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195154061.001.0001

10.1037/0012-1649.35.1.205

10.2307/1341032

10.2307/3773194

10.1016/0010-0277(86)90034-X

Fiske, 1984, Social cognition

10.1037/0022-3514.58.2.197

Carey, 1996, Modes of thought: Explorations in culture and cognition, 187

10.1037/0033-2909.117.1.21

10.1017/CBO9780511808098.032

10.1037/0033-2909.129.3.339

10.1016/j.tics.2003.08.012

10.3758/BF03193858

10.2307/1131604

10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199230167.001.0001

10.1126/science.274.5294.1926

Higgins, 1981, Personality, cognition, and social interaction, 69

10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02027.x

10.1016/0010-0277(74)90009-2

Trope, 2007, The Sage handbook of social psychology, 176

10.1037/h0034225

10.1146/annurev.ps.31.020180.002325

10.1037/a0021860

10.1017/S0140525X98001277

10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1464

10.1037/0012-1649.37.5.630

10.1017/CBO9780511808098

10.1111/j.1751-9004.2009.00214.x

10.1111/cogs.12023

Baron, 2005, Rationality and intelligence

10.1111/j.1467-9280.1990.tb00246.x

Boroditsky, 2003, Language in mind: Advances in the study of language and thought, 61, 10.7551/mitpress/4117.003.0010

Dweck, 2006, Mindset: The new psychology of success

Cimpian, The inherence heuristic across development: Systematic differences between children's and adults' explanations for everyday facts, Cognitive Psychology

10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199890712.003.0049

10.1037/0022-3514.65.5.861

10.1017/S0305000900012393

Cattell, 1986, Intelligence: Its structure, growth, and action

10.1037/0012-1649.14.5.474

10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60357-3

10.1207/s15327752jpa4803_13

10.1016/j.cogpsych.2010.06.001

10.1207/s15324834basp2701_6

10.1080/00221325.1970.10533916

10.1037/a0025274

Bloom, 2000, How children learn the meanings of words, 10.7551/mitpress/3577.001.0001

10.1080/10463280440000026

10.2307/1158253

10.1111/j.1468-0068.2010.00756.x

10.1177/0146167293193011

10.1006/jmla.1995.1005

10.1037/0022-3514.54.5.733

10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01723.x

10.1348/014466600164363

10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199672707.003.0005

10.1007/978-1-4613-9223-1_1

10.1126/science.1204493

10.1037/0033-295X.111.1.3

10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01525.x

Gil-White, 2001, Are ethnic groups biological “species” to the human brain?, Current Anthropology, 42, 515, 10.1086/321802

Kahneman, 2011, Thinking, fast and slow

10.1016/S0022-1031(77)80007-3

10.1207/s15516709cog1401_5

10.1016/j.cogpsych.2008.11.001

10.1177/0146167289153011

10.1002/9781444390933.ch30

Bloom, 2004, Descartes' baby: How the science of child development explains what makes us human

10.1111/j.1751-9004.2008.00112.x

10.1016/j.cogdev.2004.07.003

10.2307/1130860

10.1016/0022-1031(67)90034-0

Frazer, 1890, The golden bough: A study in magic and religion

10.1016/0010-0277(91)90007-Q

10.1037/0022-3514.45.3.513

10.1016/0010-0277(94)00660-D

10.1037/h0032602

10.2307/1130858

10.1037/0022-3514.35.7.485

10.1111/j.2044-8309.1994.tb01008.x

10.1037/a0015997

Gelman, 2004, Mother–child conversations about gender: Understanding the acquisition of essentialist beliefs, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 69

10.2307/1128515

10.1037/0022-3514.43.1.35

Jost, 2001, Cognitive social psychology: The Princeton symposium on the legacy and future of social cognition, 89

10.1017/CBO9780511529863.009

10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01896.x

10.1016/j.cognition.2007.01.012

10.2307/1166211

Hume, 1740, A treatise of human nature

Piaget, 1929, The child's conception of the world

10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093629

10.1016/0010-0277(94)90027-2

10.1177/0146167200265003

10.3102/00346543059004333

Murphy, 2004, The big book of concepts

Sutherland S. L. & Cimpian A. (in preparation) An inherence heuristic in explanation promotes the belief that words are well suited for their referents.

10.1023/A:1026380825208

10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01144.x

10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00071-2

10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60417-7

10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01649.x

10.1037/0033-295X.92.3.289

Stanovich, 2011, Rationality and the reflective mind

10.1037/0022-0663.89.2.342

Paoletti, 2012, Pink and blue: Telling the boys from the girls in America

10.1177/0956797612460407

Hooper, 1890, Hints on home dress-making, The Ladies' Home Journal, 7

10.1037/a0017058

10.1037/0022-3514.56.1.5

Kelley, 1967, Attribution theory in social psychology, Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 15, 192

Chen, 1999, Dual-process theories in social and cognitive psychology, 73

10.1177/0956797611429803

10.1111/j.1467-6494.1979.tb00211.x

10.1017/CBO9780511624940

10.1207/s15327078in0903_5

10.1016/j.cognition.2009.07.004

10.1016/j.cognition.2005.01.003

10.1037/0033-295X.90.4.293

10.1037/0096-3445.126.2.99

10.1037/0022-3514.32.4.605

10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1421

Lipton, 2004, Inference to the best explanation

10.1016/j.cogpsych.2009.05.001

10.1016/j.cub.2007.06.022

10.1037/0033-2909.109.3.490

10.1016/0010-0277(95)00699-0

10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.tb06238.x

10.1348/014466602165072

10.1016/j.cognition.2012.02.003

10.1037/0022-3514.94.6.1033

10.1037/a0019665

10.1080/17470216008416717

10.1037/0022-3514.50.5.869

10.1207/S15327957PSPR0502_5

Jones, 1965, Advances in experimental social psychology, vol. 2, 219, 10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60107-0

10.1017/CBO9780511808098

Anderson, 1996, Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles, 271