The influence of retrieval practice on metacognition: The contribution of analytic and non-analytic processes
Tài liệu tham khảo
Arbuckle, 1969, Discrimination of item strength at time of presentation, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 126, 10.1037/h0027455
Baumeister, 2001, Bad is stronger than good, Review of General Psychology, 5, 323, 10.1037/1089-2680.5.4.323
Butler, 2011, The hypercorrection effect persists over a week, but high confidence errors return, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 1238, 10.3758/s13423-011-0173-y
Castel, 2008, Metacognition and learning about primacy and recency effects in free recall: The utilization of intrinsic and extrinsic cues when making judgments of learning, Memory & Cognition, 36, 429, 10.3758/MC.36.2.429
Cheng, 2010, Accuracy and stability of metacognitive monitoring: A new measure, Behavior Research Methods, 42, 715, 10.3758/BRM.42.3.715
Dunlosky, 2002, Influence of practice tests on the accuracy of predicting memory performance for paired associates, sentences, and text material, 68
Flavell, 1979, Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area of cognitive developmental inquiry, American Psychologist, 34, 906, 10.1037/0003-066X.34.10.906
Fleming, 2014, How to measure metacognition, Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00443
Frederickson, 2013, Updated thinking on positivity ratios, American Psychologist, 68, 814, 10.1037/a0033584
Frederickson, 2005, Positive affect and the complex dynamics of human flourishing, American Psychologist, 60, 678, 10.1037/0003-066X.60.7.678
Gehring, 2012, The error-related negativity (ERN/Ne), 231
Grimaldi, 2010, Normative multitrial recall performance, metacognitive judgments, and retrieval latencies for Lithuanian-English paired associates, Behavior Research Methods, 42, 634, 10.3758/BRM.42.3.634
Hart, 1965, Memory and feeling-of-knowing experience, Journal of Educational Psychology, 56, 208, 10.1037/h0022263
Hartwig, 2014, The contribution of judgment scale to the unskilled-and-unaware phenomenon: How evaluating others can exaggerate over- (and under-) confidence, Memory & Cognition, 42, 164, 10.3758/s13421-013-0351-4
Kelley, 1996, Adult egocentrism: Subjective experience versus analytic bases for judgment, Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 157, 10.1006/jmla.1996.0009
Koriat, 1997, Monitoring one’s own knowledge during study: A cue-utilization approach to judgments of learning, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 126, 349, 10.1037/0096-3445.126.4.349
Koriat, 2004, Predicting one’s own forgetting: The role of experience-based and theory-based processes, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 643, 10.1037/0096-3445.133.4.643
Kornell, 2009, A stability bias in human memory: Overestimating remembering and underestimating learning, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138, 449, 10.1037/a0017350
Kornell, 2011, The ease of processing heuristic and the stability bias: Dissociating memory, memory beliefs, and memory judgments, Psychological Science, 22, 787, 10.1177/0956797611407929
Kruger, 1999, Unskilled and unaware of it: How difficulties in recognizing ones’ own incompetence lead to inflated self-assessments, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 1121, 10.1037/0022-3514.77.6.1121
Maki, 1992, Role of practice tests in the accuracy of text predictions on text material, Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 200, 10.1037/0022-0663.84.2.200
Matvey, 2001, Fluency of retrieval at study affects judgments of learning (JOLs): An analytic or nonanalytic basis for JOLs?, Memory & Cognition, 29, 222, 10.3758/BF03194916
Metcalfe, 2011, People’s hypercorrection of high-confidence errors: Did they know it all along?, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37, 437
Miller, 2014, Improving metacognitive accuracy: How failing to retrieve practice items reduces overconfidence, Consciousness and Cognition, 29, 131, 10.1016/j.concog.2014.08.008
Mueller, 2014, The font-size effect on judgments of learning: Does it exemplify fluency effects or reflect people’s beliefs about memory?, Journal of Memory and Language, 70, 1, 10.1016/j.jml.2013.09.007
Mueller, 2013, Contributions of beliefs and processing fluency to the effect of relatedness on judgments of learning, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20, 378, 10.3758/s13423-012-0343-6
Nelson, 1996, Gamma is a measure of the accuracy of predicting performance on one item relative to another item, not of the absolute performance of an individual item: Comments on Schraw (1995), Applied Cognitive Psychology, 10, 257, 10.1002/(SICI)1099-0720(199606)10:3<257::AID-ACP400>3.0.CO;2-9
Nelson, 1991, When people’s judgments of learning (JOLs) are extremely accurate at predicting subsequent recall: The “delayed-JOL” effect, Psychological Science, 2, 267, 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1991.tb00147.x
Nelson, 1990, Metamemory: A theoretical framework and new findings, Vol. 26, 125
Pierce, 2001, The postdiction superiority effect in metacomprehension of text, Memory & Cognition, 29, 62, 10.3758/BF03195741
Rhodes, 2008, Memory predictions are influenced by perceptual information: Evidence for metacognitive illusions, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137, 615, 10.1037/a0013684
Schwartz, 1994, Sources of information in metamemory: Judgments of learning and feelings of knowing, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1, 357, 10.3758/BF03213977
Serra, 2014, People use the memory for past-test heuristic as an explicit cue for judgments of learning, Memory & Cognition, 42, 1260, 10.3758/s13421-014-0431-0
Van der Helden, 2009, The importance of failure: Feedback-related negativity predicts motor learning efficiency, Cerebral Cortex, 20, 1596, 10.1093/cercor/bhp224
Vesonder, 1985, On the ability to predict one’s own responses while learning, Journal of Memory and Language, 24, 363, 10.1016/0749-596X(85)90034-8