Task switching

Trends in Cognitive Sciences - Tập 7 Số 3 - Trang 134-140 - 2003
Stephen Monsell1
1School of Psychology, University of Exeter, Exeter EX4 4QG, UK

Tóm tắt

Từ khóa


Tài liệu tham khảo

Norman, 1986, Attention to action: willed and automatic control of behaviour, Vol. 4, 1

Monsell, 1996, Control of mental processes, 93

Duncan, 1990, Goal weighting and the choice of behaviour in a complex world, Ergonomics, 33, 1265, 10.1080/00140139008925331

Cooper, 2000, Contention scheduling and the control of routine activities, Cogn. Neuropsychol., 17, 297, 10.1080/026432900380427

Logan, 1985, Executive control of thought and action, Acta Psychol., 60, 193, 10.1016/0001-6918(85)90055-1

Hommel, 2000, The prepared reflex: automaticity and control in stimulus–response translation, 247

MacLeod, 1991, Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review, Psychol. Bull., 109, 163, 10.1037/0033-2909.109.2.163

Lhermitte, 1983, ‘Utilization behaviour’ and its relation to lesions of the frontal lobes, Brain, 106, 237, 10.1093/brain/106.2.237

Goschke, 2000, Intentional reconfiguration and involuntary persistence in task set switching, 331

Monsell, 2000

Sohn, 2000, The role of prefrontal cortex and posterior parietal cortex in task switching, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A., 97, 13448, 10.1073/pnas.240460497

Kimberg, 2000, Modulation of task-related neural activity in task-switching: an fMRI study, Cogn. Brain Res., 10, 189, 10.1016/S0926-6410(00)00016-1

Rogers, 1995, The costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks, J. Exp. Psychol. Gen., 124, 207, 10.1037/0096-3445.124.2.207

Rubinstein, 2001, Executive control of cognitive processes in task switching, J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform., 27, 763, 10.1037/0096-1523.27.4.763

De Jong, 2000, An intention-activation account of residual switch costs, 357

Nieuwenhuis, 2002, Residual costs in task switching: testing the ‘failure to engage’ hypothesis, Psychonomic Bull. Rev., 9, 86, 10.3758/BF03196259

Mayr, 2000, Task-set switching and long-term memory retrieval, J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn., 26, 1124, 10.1037/0278-7393.26.5.1124

Sohn, 2001, Task preparation and task repetition: two-component model of task switching, J. Exp. Psychol. Gen., 130, 764, 10.1037/0096-3445.130.4.764

Cohen, 1990, On the control of automatic processes: a parallel distributed processing account of the Stroop effect, Psychol. Rev., 97, 332, 10.1037/0033-295X.97.3.332

Allport, 1994, Shifting intentional set: exploring the dynamic control of tasks, 421

Allport, 1999, Task-switching: Positive and negative priming of task-set, 273

Yeung, N. and Monsell, S. Switching between tasks of unequal familiarity: the role of stimulus-attribute and response-set selection. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. (in press).

Meuter, 1999, Bilingual language-switching in naming: asymmetrical costs of language selection, J. Mem. Lang., 40, 25, 10.1006/jmla.1998.2602

Monsell, 2000, Reconfiguration of task-set: is it easier to switch to the weaker task?, Psychol. Res., 63, 250, 10.1007/s004269900005

Meiran, 2000, Component processes in task switching, Cogn. Psychol., 41, 211, 10.1006/cogp.2000.0736

Ruthruff, 2001, Switching between simple cognitive tasks: the interaction of top-down and bottom-up factors, J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform., 27, 1404, 10.1037/0096-1523.27.6.1404

Mayr, 2000, Changing internal constraints on action: the role of backward inhibition, J. Exp. Psychol. Gen., 129, 4, 10.1037/0096-3445.129.1.4

Mayr, 2002, Inhibition of action rules, Psychonomic Bull. Rev., 9, 93, 10.3758/BF03196261

Allport, 2000, Task-switching, stimulus-response bindings and negative priming, 35

Waszak, F. et al. Task switching and long-term priming: role of episodic bindings in task shift costs. Cogn. Psychol. (in press).

Altmann, 2002, Forgetting to remember: the functional relationship of decay and interference, Psychol. Sci., 13, 27, 10.1111/1467-9280.00405

Hunt, 2002, Eliminating the cost of task set reconfiguration, Mem. Cogn., 30, 529, 10.3758/BF03194954

Sohn, 1998, Effects of repetition and foreknowledge in task-set recongfiguration, J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn., 26, 1445, 10.1037/0278-7393.26.6.1445

Dreisbach, 2002, Preparatory processes in the task switching paradigm: evidence from the use of probability cues, J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn., 28, 468, 10.1037/0278-7393.28.3.468

Koch, I. The role of external cues for endogenous advance reconfiguration in task switching. Psychonomic Bull. Rev. (in press).

Logan, G. and Bundesen, C. Clever homunculus: is there an endogenous act of control in the explicit task-cuing procedure. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. (in press).

Vygotski, 1962

Baddeley, 2001, Working memory and the control of action: evidence from task switching, J. Exp. Psychol. Gen., 130, 641, 10.1037/0096-3445.130.4.641

Emerson, M.J. and Miyake, A. The role of inner speech in task switching. J. Mem. Lang. (in press).

Mecklinger, 1999, Executive control functions in task switching: evidence from brain injured patients, J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol., 21, 606, 10.1076/jcen.21.5.606.873

Fitts, 1967

Monsell, S. et al. Task-set reconfiguration after a predictable or unpredictable task switch: is one trial enough? Mem. Cogn. (in press).

Gilbert, 2002, Task-switching: a PDP model, Cogn. Psychol., 44, 297, 10.1006/cogp.2001.0770

Kieras, 2000, Modern computational perspectives on executive mental processes and cognitive control: where to from here?, 681

Logan, 2001, Executive control of visual attention in dual-task situations, Psychol. Rev., 108, 393, 10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.393

Meiran, 2000, Modeling cognitive control in task-switching, Psychol. Res., 63, 234, 10.1007/s004269900004

Kramer, 1999, Task coordination and aging: explorations of executive control processes in the task-switching paradigm, Acta Psychol., 101, 339, 10.1016/S0001-6918(99)00011-6

Kray, 2000, Adult age differences in task switching, Psychol. Aging, 15, 126, 10.1037/0882-7974.15.1.126

Mayr, 2001, Age differences in the selection of mental sets: the role of inhibition, stimulus ambiguity, and response-set overlap, Psychol. Aging, 16, 96, 10.1037/0882-7974.16.1.96

Rogers, 1998, Dissociating executive mechanisms of task control following frontal lobe damage and Parkinson's disease, Brain, 121, 815, 10.1093/brain/121.5.815

Keele, 2000, Deficits of task-set in patients with left prefrontal cortex lesions, 627

Miyake, 2000, The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex ‘frontal lobe’ tasks: a latent variable analysis, Cogn. Psychol., 41, 49, 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734

Meyer, 1998, The role of dorsolateral prefronatl cortex for executive cognitive processes in task-switching, J. Cogn. Neurosci., 106

MacDonald, 2000, Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control, Science, 288, 1835, 10.1126/science.288.5472.1835

Hopfinger, 2000, Electrophysiological and neuroimaging studies of voluntary and reflexive attention, 125

Brass, 2002, The role of the frontal cortex in task preparation, Cereb. Cortex, 12, 908, 10.1093/cercor/12.9.908

Rushworth, 2002, Role of the human medial frontal cortex in task switching: a combined fMRI and TMS study, J. Neurophysiol., 87, 2577, 10.1152/jn.2002.87.5.2577

Woodworth, 1954

Jersild, 1927, Mental set and shift, Archives Psychol., 89

Biederman, 1972, Human performance in contingent information processing tasks, J. Exp. Psychol., 93, 219, 10.1037/h0032511

Shaffer, 1965, Choice reaction with variable S–R mapping, J. Exp. Psychol., 70, 284, 10.1037/h0022207

Biederman, 1973, Mental set and mental arithmetic, Mem. Cogn., 1, 383, 10.3758/BF03198124

Sudevan, 1987, The cuing and priming of cognitive operations, J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform., 13, 89, 10.1037/0096-1523.13.1.89

Meiran, 1996, Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance, J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn., 22, 1423, 10.1037/0278-7393.22.6.1423

Gopher, 2000, Switching tasks and attention policies, J. Exp. Psychol. Gen., 129, 308, 10.1037/0096-3445.129.3.308