Spatial knowledge during skilled action sequencing: Hierarchical versus nonhierarchical representations

Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics - Tập 79 - Trang 2435-2448 - 2017
Lawrence P. Behmer1, Matthew J. C. Crump1
1Department of Psychology, Brooklyn College of the City University of New York, Brooklyn, USA

Tóm tắt

Typists can type 4 to 5 keystrokes per second at around 95% accuracy, yet they appear to have poor declarative knowledge of key locations. Logan and Crump (2011, Psychology of Learning and Motivation, Vol. 54, pp. 1–27) accounted for this paradox by proposing that typing is hierarchically organized into two loops, with an outer loop that transforms sentences into words and passes each word, one at a time, to an inner loop that transforms each word into its constituent keystrokes; however, the nature of the inner loop’s spatial knowledge is not well understood. Key locations may be learned through the experiences of locating and traversing between keys. In daily life, people tend to type structured language, and, as a consequence, certain keys and key-to-key transitions are experienced more frequently than others. Here, we asked whether or not this knowledge is structured hierarchically. For example, knowledge of key locations may be nested within representations of words, or the inner loop may rely on knowledge that is independent from higher level structures. To test this, we had people type English, English-like, and random strings during normal, partially occluded, and occluded typing. In both partially occluded and occluded typing, error rates were higher while typing random strings compared to English and English-like strings, whereas there was no difference in error rates between English and English-like strings. This suggests that typists’ spatial knowledge of the keyboard is not driven by hierarchical word-level representations, but instead is likely driven by a collection of individual processes, such as knowledge of the sequential structure of language acquired by typing more frequently occurring letters.

Tài liệu tham khảo

Behmer, L. P., Jr., & Crump, M. J. C. (2017a). Crunching big data with finger tips: How typists tune their performance toward the statistics of natural language. In M. Jones (Ed.), Big data in cognitive science (pp. 1–28). London: Routledge. Behmer, L. P., Jr., & Crump, M. J. C. (2017b). The dynamic range of response set activation during action sequencing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 43(3), 537–554. Byrne, R. W. (1982). Geographical knowledge and orientation. In A. W. Ellis (Ed.), Normality and pathology in cognitive functions (pp. 239–264). London: Academic Press. Crump, M. J. C., & Logan, G. D. (2010). Warning: This keyboard will deconstruct—The role of the keyboard in skilled typewriting. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17, 394–399. doi:10.3758/PBR.17.3.394 Gentner, D., Larochelle, S., & Grudin, J. (1988). Lexical, sublexical, and peripheral effects in skilled typewriting. Cognitive Psychology, 20, 524–548. Hintzman, D. L., O’Dell, C. S., & Arndt, D. R. (1981). Orientation in cognitive maps. Cognitive Psychology, 13, 149–206. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(81)90007-4 Ishikawa, T., & Montello, D. R. (2006). Spatial knowledge acquisition from direct experience in the environment: Individual differences in the development of metric knowledge and the integration of separately learned places. Cognitive Psychology, 52, 93–129. Kosslyn, S. M. (1975). Information representation in visual images. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 341–370. Kosslyn, S. M., & Pomerantz, J. R. (1977). Imagery, propositions, and the form of internal representations. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 52–76. Kosslyn, S. M., & Shwartz, S. P. (1977). A simulation of visual imagery. Cognitive Science, 1, 265–295. Liu, X., Crump, M. J. C., & Logan, G. D. (2010). Do you know where your fingers have been? Explicit knowledge of the spatial layout of the keyboard in skilled typists. Memory & Cognition, 38(4), 474–484. Logan, G. D. (1988). Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychological Review, 95(4), 492–527. Logan, G. D., & Crump, M. J. C. (2011). Hierarchical control of cognitive processes. In B. H. Ross (Ed.), Psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 54, pp. 1–27). Amsterdam: Elsevier. Maguire, E. A., Frackowiak, R. S., & Frith, C. D. (1997). Recalling routes around London: Activation of the right hippocampus in taxi drivers. The Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 7103–7110. McNamara, T. P. (1986). Mental representations of spatial relations. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 87–121. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(86)90016-2 McNamara, T. P., Ratcliff, R., & McKoon, G. (1984). The mental representation of knowledge acquired from maps. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10, 723–732. Pinet, S., Ziegler, J. C., & Alario, F. X. (2016). Typing is writing: Linguistic properties modulate typing execution. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23, 1898–1906. Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1973). What the mind’s eye tells the mind’s brain: A critique of mental imagery. Psychological Bulletin, 80, 1–24. Rosenbaum, D. A., Loukopoulos, L. D., Meulenbroek, R. G., Vaughan, J., & Engelbrecht, S. E. (1995). Planning reaches by evaluating stored postures. Psychological Review, 102(1), 28–67. Scaltritti, M., Arfe, B., Torrance, M., & Peressotti, F. (2016). Typing pictures: Linguistic processing cascades into finger movements. Cognition, 156, 16–29. Shaffer, L. H., & Hardwick, J. (1968). Typing performance as a function of text. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 20, 360–369. doi:10.1080/14640746808400175 Shelton, A. L., & McNamara, T. P. (2001). Systems of spatial reference in human memory. Cognitive Psychology, 43, 274–310. Siegel, A. W., & White, S. H. (1975). The development of spatial representations of large-scale environments. In H. W. Reese (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 10, pp. 9–55). Bingley: JAI. Snyder, K. M., Ashitaka, Y., Shimada, H., Ulrich, J. E., & Logan, G. D. (2014). What skilled typists don’t know about the QWERTY keyboard. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76, 162–171. doi:10.3758/s13414-013-0548-4 Snyder, K. M., Logan, G. D., & Yamaguchi, M. (2015). Watch what you type: The role of visual feedback from the screen and hands in skilled typewriting. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 77, 282–292. doi:10.3758/s13414-014-0756-6 Stevens, A., & Coupe, P. (1978). Distortions in judged spatial relations. Cognitive Psychology, 10, 422–437. Thorndyke, P. W., & Hayes-Roth, B. (1982). Differences in spatial knowledge acquired from maps and navigation. Cognitive Psychology, 14, 560–589. Tversky, B. (1993). Cognitive maps, cognitive collages, and spatial mental models. In A. U. Frank & I. Campari (Eds.), Spatial information theory: A theoretical basis for GIS [COSIT 1993: Lecture notes in computer science] (Vol. 716, pp. 14–24). Berlin: Springer. Tversky, B. (2000). Levels and structure of spatial knowledge. In R. Kitchin & S. Freundschuh (Eds.), Cognitive mapping: Past, present and future (pp. 24–43). London: Routledge. Van Selst, M. V., & Jolicoeur, P. (1994). A solution to the effect of sample size on outlier elimination. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47(3), 631–650. Wang, R. F., & Brockmole, J. R. (2003). Human navigation in nested environments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 398–404. Wilson, M. (1987). UWA psychology: MRC psycholinguistic database (Dict interface). Retrieved from http://websites.psychology.uwa.edu.au/school/MRCDatabase/uwa_mrc.htm