Socioeconomic Status and Childhood Executive Function: Differing Conceptualizations, Diverse Assessments, and Decontextualized Investigations
Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science - Trang 1-16 - 2022
Tóm tắt
Developing the ability to regulate actions, thoughts, and emotions is necessary for successfully engaging in goal-directed activities, which form the essence of success in many life situations. In this regard, executive function (EF), as an aspect of these top-down control processes, has been the subject of inquiry in many developmental studies aiming to identify its link with various other aspects of a child’s life, including family socioeconomic status (SES). This article presents a critical look at the association between family SES and childhood EF by reviewing studies that provide contrasting perspectives compared to the generally reported positive correlation between these two constructs. We first address the various aspects of assessing SES and childhood EF, emphasizing their multifaceted nature. Next, we discuss the interplays between different components of these two constructs, especially as it unfolds in various cultural contexts. Finally, we conclude by discussing mainly neglected lines of research that could further improve our understanding of the extent of socioeconomic impacts on child development, specifically regarding executive function. Considering these lines of research is a necessary step to a more accurate analysis of the link between socioeconomic factors and childhood executive function.
Tài liệu tham khảo
Allan, N. P., & Lonigan, C. J. (2011). Examining the Dimensionality of Effortful Control in Preschool Children and Its Relation to Academic and Socioemotional Indicators. Developmental Psychology, 47(4), 905–915. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023748
Arain, M., Haque, M., Johal, L., Mathur, P., Nel, W., Rais, A. … Sharma, S. (2013). Maturation of the adolescent brain. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 9, 449
Ardila, A. (2005). Cultural values underlying psychometric cognitive testing. Neuropsychology Review, 15(4), 185–195. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-005-9180-y
Backer-Grøndahl, A., & Nærde, A. (2017). Self-regulation in early childhood: The role of siblings, center care and socioeconomic status. Social Development, 26(3), 530–544. https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12216
Baggetta, P., & Alexander, P. A. (2016). Conceptualization and Operationalization of Executive Function. Mind, Brain, and Education, 10(1), 10–33. https://doi.org/10.1111/mbe.12100
Blair, C., Raver, C. C., & Berry, D. J. (2014). Two approaches to estimating the effect of parenting on the development of executive function in early childhood. Developmental Psychology, 50(2), 554
Carlson, S. M., Davis, A. C., & Leach, J. G. (2005). Less Is More - Executive function and symbolic representation in preschool children. Psychological Science, 16(8), 609–616. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01583.x
Carlson, S. M., & Moses, L. J. (2001). Individual Differences in Inhibitory Control and Children’s Theory of Mind. Child Development, 72(4), 1032–1053. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00333
Conway, A., Waldfogel, J., & Wang, Y. (2018). Parent education and income gradients in children’s executive functions at kindergarten entry. Children and Youth Services Review, 91, 329–337
Conway, A., Waldfogel, J., & Wang, Y. (2019). Disparities in kindergarteners’ executive functions at kindergarten entry: Relations with parenting and child care. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 48, 267–283. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2019.03.009
Dahlman, S., Bäckström, P., Bohlin, G., & Frans, Ö. (2013). Cognitive abilities of street children: Low-SES Bolivian boys with and without experience of living in the street. Child Neuropsychology, 19(5), 540–556. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2012.731499
Daneri, M. P., Blair, C., & Kuhn, L. J. (2019). Maternal language and child vocabulary mediate relations between socioeconomic status and executive function during early childhood. Child Development, 90(6), 2001–2018
de Castro, L. R. (2020). Why global? Children and childhood from a decolonial perspective. Childhood, 27(1), 48–62. https://doi.org/10.1177/0907568219885379
Dearing, E. (2004). The developmental implications of restrictive and supportive parenting across neighborhoods and ethnicities: Exceptions are the rule. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 25(5), 555–575. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2004.08.007
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135–168
Distefano, R., Galinsky, E., McClelland, M. M., Zelazo, P. D., & Carlson, S. M. (2018). Autonomy-supportive parenting and associations with child and parent executive function. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 58, 77–85. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2018.04.007
Doebel, S. (2020). Rethinking Executive Function and Its Development. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 15(4), 942–956. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691620904771
Domenech RodrÍguez, M. M., Donovick, M. R., & Crowley, S. L. (2009). Parenting styles in a cultural context: Observations of protective parenting in first-generation Latinos. Family Process, 48(2), 195–210. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1545-5300.2009.01277.x
Duncan, G. J., & Magnuson, K. (2012). Socioeconomic status and cognitive functioning: moving from correlation to causation. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 3(3), 377–386
Eisenberg, I. W., Bissett, P. G., Enkavi, Z., Li, A., MacKinnon, J., Marsch, D. P., L. A., & Poldrack, R. A. (2019). Uncovering the structure of self-regulation through data-driven ontology discovery. Nature Communications, 10(1), 1–13https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-019-10301-1
Farah, M. J. (2017). The neuroscience of socioeconomic status: Correlates, causes, and consequences. Neuron, 96(1), 56–71
Finch, J. E., & Obradović, J. (2017). Unique effects of socioeconomic and emotional parental challenges on children’s executive functions. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 52, 126–137
Fujisawa, K. K., Todo, N., & Ando, J. (2017). Genetic and environmental influences on the development and stability of executive functions in children of preschool age: A longitudinal study of Japanese twins. Infant and Child Development, 26(3), e1994
Gerstadt, C. L., Hong, Y. J., & Diamond, A. (1994). The relationship between cognition and action: performance of children 3 1 2–7 years old on a stroop- like day-night test. Cognition, 53(2), 129–153. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(94)90068-X
Grabell, A. S., Olson, S. L., Miller, A. L., Kessler, D. A., Felt, B., Kaciroti, N. … Tardif, T. (2014). The impact of culture on physiological processes of emotion regulation: a comparison of US and Chinese preschoolers. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12227
Hackman, D. A., Gallop, R., Evans, G. W., & Farah, M. J. (2015). Socioeconomic status and executive function: Developmental trajectories and mediation. Developmental Science, 18(5), 686–702
Haft, S. L., & Hoeft, F. (2017). Poverty’s Impact on Children’s Executive Functions: Global Considerations. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 2017(158), 69–79. https://doi.org/10.1002/cad.20220
Heng, J., Quan, J., Sim, L. W., Sanmugam, S., Broekman, B., Bureau, J. F. … Rifkin-Graboi, A. (2018). The role of ethnicity and socioeconomic status in Southeast Asian mothers’ parenting sensitivity. Attachment and Human Development, 20(1), 24–42. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2017.1365912
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 61–83. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X0999152X
Holochwost, S. J., Gariépy, J. L., Propper, C. B., Neblett, N. G., Volpe, V., Neblett, E., & Mills-Koonce, W. R. (2016). Sociodemographic risk, parenting, and executive functions in early childhood: The role of ethnicity. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 36, 537–549. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2016.02.001
Howard, S. J., Cook, C. J., Everts, L., Melhuish, E., Scerif, G., Norris, S. … Draper, C. E. (2019). Challenging socioeconomic status: A cross-cultural comparison of early executive function.Developmental Science, e12854
Ichoku, H. E. (2011). On the use of socioeconomic status indicators in the analysis of health inequalities and poverty in Africa. Journal of International Development, 23(7), 1004–1012. https://doi.org/10.1002/jid.1697
John, A. M. S., & Tarullo, A. R. (2019). Neighbourhood chaos moderates the association of socioeconomic status and child executive functioning. Infant and Child Development
Keller, H., Yovsi, R., Borke, J., Kartner, J., Jensen, H., & Papaligoura, Z. (2004). Developmental Consequences of Early Parenting Experiences: Self-Recognition and Self-Regulation in Three Cultural Communities. Child Development, 75(6), 1745–1760. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00814.x
Kim, S., Nordling, J. K., Yoon, J. E., Boldt, L. J., & Kochanska, G. (2013). Effortful control in “hot” and “cool” tasks differentially predicts children’s behavior problems and academic performance. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41(1), 43–56. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-012-9661-4
Kochanska, G., Murray, K., Jacques, T. Y., Koenig, A. L., & Vandegeest, K. A. (1996). Inhibitory Control in Young Children and Its Role in Emerging Internalization. Child Development, 67(2), 490–507. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01747.x
Lamm, B., Keller, H., Teiser, J., Gudi, H., Yovsi, R. D., Freitag, C. … Lohaus, A. (2018). Waiting for the Second Treat: Developing Culture-Specific Modes of Self-Regulation. Child Development, 89(3), e261–e277. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12847
Last, B. S., Lawson, G. M., Breiner, K., Steinberg, L., & Farah, M. J. (2018). Childhood socioeconomic status and executive function in childhood and beyond. PloS One, 13(8), e0202964. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0202964
Lawson, G. M., Hook, C. J., & Farah, M. J. (2018). A meta-analysis of the relationship between socioeconomic status and executive function performance among children.Developmental Science, 21(2), e12529. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12529
McClelland, M. M., & Cameron, C. E. (2019). Developing together: The role of executive function and motor skills in children’s early academic lives. Early Childhood Research Quarterly (Vol, 46, 142–151. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.03.014. JAI
McCoy, D. C. (2019). Measuring Young Children’s Executive Function and Self-Regulation in Classrooms and Other Real-World Settings. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 22(1), 63–74. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567-019-00285-1
McCoy, D. C., Zuilkowski, S. S., & Fink, G. (2015). Poverty, physical stature, and cognitive skills: Mechanisms underlying children’s school enrollment in zambia. Developmental Psychology, 51(5), 600–614. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038924
Merz, E. C., Landry, S. H., Montroy, J. J., & Williams, J. M. (2017). Bidirectional Associations Between Parental Responsiveness and Executive Function During Early Childhood. Social Development, 26(3), 591–609. https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12204
Merz, E. C., Wiltshire, C. A., & Noble, K. G. (2019). Socioeconomic Inequality and the Developing Brain: Spotlight on Language and Executive Function. Child Development Perspectives, 13(1), 15–20
Mischel, W., Ebbesen, E. B., & Raskoff Zeiss, A. (1972). Cognitive and attentional mechanisms in delay of gratification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 21(2), 204–218. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0032198
Mistry, J., & Dutta, R. (2015). Human Development and Culture. In In W. F. Overton, P. C. M. Molenaar, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of Child Psychology and Developmental Science (pp. 1–38). John Wiley & Sons, Inc. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy110
Molenaar, P. C. M., & Nesselroade, J. R. (2015). Systems Methods for Developmental Research. In W. F. Overton, P. C. M. Molenaar, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.),Handbook of Child Psychology and Developmental Science: Theory and method (pp. 652–682). John Wiley & Sons, Inc. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy117
Morelli, G., Bard, K., Chaudhary, N., Gottlieb, A., Keller, H., Murray, M. … Vicedo, M. (2018). Bringing the Real World Into Developmental Science: A Commentary on Weber, Fernald, and Diop (2017). Child Development, 89(6), e594–e603. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13115
Mousavi, S. Z., & Gharibzadeh, S. (2021). Growing up in a challenging environment: A cultural analysis of self-regulation development in poverty. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 00(00), 1–18. https://doi.org/10.1080/17405629.2021.1928490
Mulder, H., van Ravenswaaij, H., Verhagen, J., Moerbeek, M., & Leseman, P. P. M. (2019). The process of early self-control: an observational study in two- and three-year-olds. Metacognition and Learning, 14(3), 239–264. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-019-09199-3
Neville, H. J., Stevens, C., Pakulak, E., Bell, T. A., Fanning, J., Klein, S., & Isbell, E. (2013). Family-based training program improves brain function, cognition, and behavior in lower socioeconomic status preschoolers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 110(29), 12138–12143. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1304437110
Nigg, J. T. (2017). Annual Research Review: On the relations among self-regulation, self-control, executive functioning, effortful control, cognitive control, impulsivity, risk-taking, and inhibition for developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 58(4), 361–383. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12675
Nigg, J. T., Wong, M. M., Martel, M. M., Jester, J. M., Puttler, L. I., Glass, J. M. … Zucker, R. A. (2006). Poor response inhibition as a predictor of problem drinking and illicit drug use in adolescents at risk for alcoholism and other substance use disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 45(4), 468–475. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.chi.0000199028.76452.a9
Oakes, J. M., & Rossi, P. H. (2003). The measurement of SES in health research: Current practice and steps toward a new approach. Social Science and Medicine, 56(4), 769–784. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0277-9536(02)00073-4
Obradović, J., & Willoughby, M. T. (2019). Studying Executive Function Skills in Young Children in Low- and Middle-Income Countries: Progress and Directions. Child Development Perspectives, 13(4), 227–234. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12349
Obradovíc, J., Yousafzai, A. K., Finch, J. E., & Rasheed, M. A. (2016). Maternal scaffolding and home stimulation: Key mediators of early intervention effects on children’s cognitive development. Developmental Psychology, 52(9), 1409–1421. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000182
Overton, W. F. (2015). Processes, Relations, and Relational-Developmental-Systems. In W. F. Overton, P. C. M. Molenaar, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of Child Psychology and Developmental Science: Theory and method (pp. 9–62). John Wiley & Sons, Inc. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy102
Perone, S., Simmering, Vanessa, R., & Buss, A. T. (2020). A dynamical reconceptualization of executive function development. Perspectives on Psychological Science,16(6), 1198–1208. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691620966792
Peterson, E., & Welsh, M. C. (2014). The development of hot and cool executive functions in childhood and adolescence: are we getting warmer?. In S. Goldstein, & J. A. Naglieri (Eds.), Handbook of executive functioning (pp. 45–65). Springer
Pluck, G., Banda-Cruz, D. R., Andrade-Guimaraes, M. V., & Trueba, A. F. (2018). Socioeconomic deprivation and the development of neuropsychological functions: A study with “street children” in Ecuador. Child Neuropsychology, 24(4), 510–523. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2017.1294150
Poon, K. (2018). Hot and cool executive functions in adolescence: Development and contributions to important developmental outcomes. Frontiers in Psychology, 8(JAN), 2311. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02311
Richland, L. E., & Burchinal, M. R. (2013). Early Executive Function Predicts Reasoning Development. Psychological Science, 24(1), 87–92. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612450883
Rogoff, B. (2003). The cultural nature of human development. In Oxford University Press. https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2003-04956-000
Rogoff, B., Dahl, A., & Callanan, M. (2018). The importance of understanding children’s lived experience. Developmental Review, 50(June), 5–15. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2018.05.006
Roy, A. L., Isaia, A., & Li-Grining, C. P. (2019). Making meaning from money: Subjective social status and young children’s behavior problems. Journal of Family Psychology, 33(2), 240
Rybanska, V., McKay, R., Jong, J., & Whitehouse, H. (2018). Rituals Improve Children’s Ability to Delay Gratification. Child Development, 89(2), 349–359. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12762
Sarma, U. A., & Mariam Thomas, T. (2020). Breaking the limits of executive functions: Towards a sociocultural perspective. In Culture and Psychology (Vol. 26, Issue 3, pp. 358–368). https://doi.org/10.1177/1354067X19898673
Schmitt, S. A., Korucu, I., Purpura, D. J., Whiteman, S., Zhang, C., & Yang, F. (2019). Exploring cross-cultural variations in the development of executive function for preschoolers from low and high socioeconomic families. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 43(3), 212–220. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025418785469
Sobeh, J., & Spijkers, W. (2013). Development of neuropsychological functions of attention in two cultures: A cross-cultural study of attentional performances of Syrian and German children of pre-school and school age. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 10(3), 318–336. https://doi.org/10.1080/17405629.2012.674761
Thorell, L. B., Veleiro, A., Siu, A. F. Y., & Mohammadi, H. (2013). Examining the relation between ratings of executive functioning and academic achievement: Findings from a cross-cultural study. Child Neuropsychology, 19(6), 630–638. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2012.727792
Tran, C. D., Arredondo, M. M., & Yoshida, H. (2019). Early executive function: The influence of culture and bilingualism. Bilingualism, 22(4), 714–732. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728918000160
Turbeville, A., Aber, J. L., Weinberg, S. L., Richter, L., & van Heerden, A. (2019). The relationship between multidimensional economic well-being and children’s mental health, physical health, and executive function development in South Africa. Developmental Science, 22(5), e12846. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12846
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, National Institutes of Health, Sciences, O. of B. and S. S. R. (n.d.). Measuring Socioeconomic Status. https://obssr.od.nih.gov/sites/obssr/files/Measuring-Socioeconomic-Status.pdf
Ursache, A., Noble, K. G., & Blair, C. (2015). Socioeconomic status, subjective social status, and perceived stress: Associations with stress physiology and executive functioning. Behavioral Medicine, 41(3), 145–154
von Eye, A., Bergman, L. R., & Hsieh, C. A. (2015). Person-Oriented Methodological Approaches. In W. F. Overton, P. C. M. Molenaar, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of Child Psychology and Developmental Science: Theory and method (pp. 789–841). John Wiley & Sons, Inc. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy121
Vrantsidis, D. M., Clark, C. A. C., Chevalier, N., Espy, K. A., & Wiebe, S. A. (2019). Socioeconomic status and executive function in early childhood: Exploring proximal mechanisms.Developmental Science, e12917
Zelazo, P. D. (2015). Executive function: Reflection, iterative reprocessing, complexity, and the developing brain. Developmental Review, 38, 55–68
Zelazo, P. D., Müller, U., Frye, D., Marcovitch, S., Argitis, G., Boseovski, J. … Sutherland, A. (2003). The development of executive function in early childhood. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 68(3), vii–137. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/14723273