Simple heuristics and rules of thumb: Where psychologists and behavioural biologists might meet

Behavioural Processes - Tập 69 Số 2 - Trang 97-124 - 2005
J. Hutchinson1, Gerd Gigerenzer1
1Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Lentzeallee 94, 14195 Berlin, Germany

Tóm tắt

Từ khóa


Tài liệu tham khảo

Anderson, 1990

Anderson, 2004, An integrated theory of mind, Psych. Rev., 111, 1036, 10.1037/0033-295X.111.4.1036

Arkes, 1999, The sunk cost and concorde effects: are humans less rational than lower animals?, Psych. Bull., 125, 591, 10.1037/0033-2909.125.5.591

Axelrod, 1981, The evolution of cooperation, Science, 211, 1390, 10.1126/science.7466396

Balda, 1992, Long-term spatial memory in Clark's nutcracker, Nucifruga columbiana, Anim. Behav., 44, 761, 10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80302-1

Balkovsky, 2002, Olfactory search at high Reynolds number, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A., 99, 12589, 10.1073/pnas.192393499

Barta, 2002, Sexual conflict about parental care: the role of reserves, Am. Nat., 159, 687, 10.1086/339995

Basolo, 2002, The confirmation of a female preference for a composite male trait in green swordtails, Anim. Behav., 63, 469, 10.1006/anbe.2001.1933

Bateson, 2003, Context-dependent foraging decisions in rufous hummingbirds, Proc. R. Soc. Lond., 270, 1271, 10.1098/rspb.2003.2365

Baum, 2001, Hummingbird foraging behavior in different patch types: simulation of alternative strategies, Ecol. Mod., 137, 201, 10.1016/S0304-3800(00)00436-1

Bélisle, 1997, The effects of a limited memory capacity on foraging behavior, Theor. Pop. Biol., 52, 78, 10.1006/tpbi.1997.1319

Benhamou, 1992, Efficiency of area-concentrated searching behaviour in a continuous patchy environment, J. Theor. Biol., 159, 67, 10.1016/S0022-5193(05)80768-4

Bonduriansky, 2003, Layered sexual selection: a comparative analysis of sexual behaviour within an assemblage of piophilid flies, Can. J. Zool., 81, 479, 10.1139/z03-031

Bookstaber, 1985, On the optimality of coarse behavior rules, J. Theor. Biol., 116, 161, 10.1016/S0022-5193(85)80262-9

Bradbeer, 1988

Brancazio, 1985, Looking into Chapman's homer: the physics of judging a fly ball, Am. J. Phys., 53, 849, 10.1119/1.14350

Breiman, 1984

Brewer, 1996

Bröder, 2000, Assessing the empirical validity of the “Take-the-Best” heuristic as a model of human probabilistic inference, J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn., 26, 1332, 10.1037/0278-7393.26.5.1332

Bröder, 2003, Decision making with the “adaptive toolbox”: influence of environmental structure, intelligence, and working memory load, J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn., 29, 611, 10.1037/0278-7393.29.4.611

Bröder, 2003, Take The Best versus simultaneous feature matching: probabilistic inferences form memory and effects of representation format, J. Exp. Psychol. Gen., 132, 277, 10.1037/0096-3445.132.2.277

Buntine, 1992

Burke, 2003, An evolved spatial memory bias in a nectar-feeding bird?, Anim. Behav., 66, 695, 10.1006/anbe.2003.2246

Calkins, 2003, Mate choice for multiple ornaments in the California quail, Callipepla californica, Anim. Behav., 65, 69, 10.1006/anbe.2002.2041

Camazine, 2001

Candolin, 2003, The use of multiple cues in mate choice, Biol. Rev., 78, 575, 10.1017/S1464793103006158

Charnov, 1976, Optimal foraging, the marginal value theorem, Theor. Pop. Biol., 9, 129, 10.1016/0040-5809(76)90040-X

Chater, 1999, Ten years of the rational analysis of cognition, Trends Cogn. Sci., 3, 57, 10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01273-X

Chater, 2003, Fast, frugal, and rational: how rational norms explain behavior, Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process., 90, 63, 10.1016/S0749-5978(02)00508-3

Cheverton, 1985, Optimal foraging: constraints and currencies, 109

Cresswell, 2000, Evidence for a rule governing the avoidance of superfluous escape flights, Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B, 267, 733, 10.1098/rspb.2000.1064

Czerlinski, 1999, How good are simple heuristics?, 97

Davies, 1981, Owners and satellites: the economics of territory defence in the pied wagtail, Motacilla alba, J. Anim. Ecol., 50, 157, 10.2307/4038

Davis, 1999, Environment quality predicts parental provisioning decisions, Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B, 266, 1791, 10.1098/rspb.1999.0848

Dawes, 1979, The robust beauty of improper linear models in decision making, Am. Psychol., 34, 571, 10.1037/0003-066X.34.7.571

Dawkins, 1982

Detrain, 2002, Complexity of environment and parsimony of decision rules in insect societies, Biol. Bull., 202, 268, 10.2307/1543478

de Vries, 2002, Self-organization in collective honeybee foraging: emergence of symmetry breaking, cross inhibition and equal harvest-rate distribution, Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 51, 557, 10.1007/s00265-002-0454-6

Dhami, 2003, Psychological models of professional decision making, Psychol. Sci., 14, 175, 10.1111/1467-9280.01438

Dieckmann, A., Todd, P.M., 2004. Simple ways to construct search orders. In: Forbus, K., Gentner, D., Regier, T. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 26th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ, pp. 309–314.

Driessen, 1999, Patch departure mechanisms and optimal host exploitation in an insect parasitoid, J. Anim. Ecol., 68, 445, 10.1046/j.1365-2656.1999.00296.x

1998

Dusenbery, 2001, Performance of basic strategies for following gradients in two dimensions, J. Theor. Biol., 208, 345, 10.1006/jtbi.2000.2224

Enquist, 1990, A test of the sequential assessment game: fighting in the cichlid fish Nannacara anomala, Anim. Behav., 10, 1, 10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80660-8

Fairhurst, 2003, Temporal landmarks: proximity prevails, Anim. Cogn., 6, 113, 10.1007/s10071-003-0169-8

Fantino, 1998, Judgement and decision making: behavioral approaches, Behav. Anal., 21, 203, 10.1007/BF03391964

Fantino, 1985, Choice, optimal foraging, and the delay-reduction hypothesis, Behav. Brain. Sci., 8, 315, 10.1017/S0140525X00020847

Fawcett, T.W., 2003. Multiple cues and variation in mate-choice behaviour. D.Phil. Thesis. University of Cambridge.

Fawcett, 2003, Optimal assessment of multiple cues, Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B, 270, 1637, 10.1098/rspb.2003.2328

Fortin, 2003, Searching behavior and use of sampling information by free-ranging bison (Bos bison), Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 54, 194, 10.1007/s00265-003-0599-y

Fraenkel, 1940

Franks, 2003, Strategies for choosing between alternatives with different attributes: exemplified by house-hunting ants, Anim. Behav., 65, 215, 10.1006/anbe.2002.2032

Gibson, 1996, Female choice in sage grouse: the roles of attraction and active comparison, Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 39, 55, 10.1007/s002650050266

Gigerenzer, 2000

Gigerenzer, 2004, Fast and frugal heuristics: the tools of bounded rationality, 62

Gigerenzer, 1996, Reasoning the fast and frugal way: models of bounded rationality, Psychol. Rev., 103, 650, 10.1037/0033-295X.103.4.650

Gigerenzer, 1999, Betting on one good reason: the Take The Best heuristic, 75

2001

Gigerenzer, 1999

Gill, 1988, Trapline foraging by hermit hummingbirds: competition for an undefended, renewable resource, Ecology, 69, 1933, 10.2307/1941170

2002

Goldstein, 2002, Models of ecological rationality: the recognition heuristic, Psychol. Rev., 109, 75, 10.1037/0033-295X.109.1.75

Gould, 1988

Gould, 1991

Gould, 1979, The spandrels of San Marco and the Panglossian paradigm: a critique of the adaptationist programme, Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B, 205, 581, 10.1098/rspb.1979.0086

Goulson, 2000, Why do pollinators visit proportionally fewer flowers in large patches?, Oikos, 91, 485, 10.1034/j.1600-0706.2000.910309.x

Grafen, 1990, Biological signals as handicaps, J. Theor. Biol., 144, 517, 10.1016/S0022-5193(05)80088-8

Green, 1984, Stopping rules for optimal foragers, Am. Nat., 123, 30, 10.1086/284184

Hammerstein, 2003, Why is reciprocity so rare in social animals? A protestant appeal, 83

Hankinson, 2003, Avoiding a compromise between sexual selection and species recognition: female swordtail fish assess multiple species-specific cues, Behav. Ecol., 14, 282, 10.1093/beheco/14.2.282

Hasson, 1991, Sexual displays as amplifiers: practical examples with an emphasis on feather decorations, Behav. Ecol., 2, 189, 10.1093/beheco/2.3.189

Hauert, 2002, Simple adaptive strategy wins the prisoner's dilemma, J. Theor. Biol., 218, 261, 10.1006/jtbi.2002.3072

Hebets, 2005, Attention-altering signal interactions in the multimodal courtship display of the wolf spider Schizocosa uetzi, Behav. Ecol., 16, 75, 10.1093/beheco/arh133

Henrich, 1998, The evolution of conformist transmission and the emergence of between-group differences, Evol. Hum. Behav., 19, 215, 10.1016/S1090-5138(98)00018-X

Hertwig, 1999, Quick estimation: letting the environment do the work, 209

Hertwig, 2003, More is not always better: the benefits of cognitive limits, 213

Hill, 1999, Mate choice based on static versus dynamic secondary sexual traits in the dark-eyed junco, Behav. Ecol., 10, 91, 10.1093/beheco/10.1.91

Hills, 2002, Time's crooked arrow: optimal foraging and rate-biased time perception, Anim. Behav., 64, 589, 10.1006/anbe.2002.3070

Hoffmann, 2000, Twenty years on: the evolution of cooperation revisited, J. Artif. Soc. Soc. Simul., 3

Hoffrage, 2000, Hindsight bias: a by-product of knowledge updating?, J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn., 26, 566, 10.1037/0278-7393.26.3.566

Houston, 1982, Some learning rules for acquiring information, 140

Hutchinson, 2004, Mate choice when males are in patches: optimal strategies and good rules of thumb, J. Theor. Biol., 231, 129, 10.1016/j.jtbi.2004.06.009

Hutchinson, 1993, Song, sexual selection, starvation and strategic handicaps, Anim. Behav., 45, 1153, 10.1006/anbe.1993.1139

Iwasa, 1981, Prey distribution as a factor determining the choice of optimal foraging strategy, Am. Nat., 117, 710, 10.1086/283754

Janetos, 1980, Strategies of female mate choice: a theoretical analysis, Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 7, 107, 10.1007/BF00299515

Janetos, 1981, Imperfectly optimal animals, Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 9, 203, 10.1007/BF00302939

Jennions, 1997, Variation in mate choice and mating preferences: a review of causes and consequences, Biol. Rev., 72, 283, 10.1017/S0006323196005014

Johnson, 1989, When choice models fail: compensatory models in negatively correlated environments, J. Market. Res., 26, 255, 10.2307/3172899

Kacelnik, 1992, Psychological mechanisms and the Marginal Value Theorem: effect of variability in travel time on patch exploitation, Anim. Behav., 43, 313, 10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80226-X

Kahneman, 1979, Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under risk, Econometrica, 47, 263, 10.2307/1914185

Kahneman, 1996, On the reality of cognitive illusions, Psychol. Rev., 103, 582, 10.1037/0033-295X.103.3.582

Kamil, 1981

Kareev, 2000, Seven (indeed, plus or minus two) and the detection of correlations, Psychol. Rev., 107, 397, 10.1037/0033-295X.107.2.397

Karsai, 2000, Optimality of cell arrangement and rules of thumb of cell initiation in Polistes dominulus: a modeling approach, Behav. Ecol., 11, 387, 10.1093/beheco/11.4.387

Keasar, 2002, Bees in two-armed bandit situations: foraging choices and possible decision mechanisms, Behav. Ecol., 13, 757, 10.1093/beheco/13.6.757

Kennedy, 1983, Zigzagging and casting as a programmed response to wind-borne odour: a review, Physiol. Entomol., 8, 109, 10.1111/j.1365-3032.1983.tb00340.x

Kodric-Brown, 2001, Female choice in the guppy (Poecilia reticulata): the interaction between male color and display, Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 50, 346, 10.1007/s002650100374

Körding, 2004, Bayesian integration in sensorimotor learning, Nature, 427, 244, 10.1038/nature02169

Krebs, 1977, Optimal prey selection in the great tit (Parus major), Anim. Behav., 25, 30, 10.1016/0003-3472(77)90064-1

Krebs, 1983, Perspectives in optimal foraging, 165

Künzler, 2001, Female preferences for single and combined traits in computer animated stickleback males, Behav. Ecol., 12, 681, 10.1093/beheco/12.6.681

1985

Lifjeld, 1988, Female pied flycatchers Ficedula hypoleuca choose male characteristics in homogeneous habitats, Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 22, 27, 10.1007/BF00395695

Lima, 1994, Collective detection of predatory attack by birds in the absence of alarm signals, J. Avian Biol., 25, 319, 10.2307/3677279

Logue, 1988, Research on self-control: an integrating framework, Behav. Brain. Sci., 11, 665, 10.1017/S0140525X00053978

Luttbeg, 1996, A comparative Bayes tactic for mate assessment and choice, Behav. Ecol., 7, 451, 10.1093/beheco/7.4.451

MacKay, 1992, Bayesian interpolation, Neural Comput., 4, 415, 10.1162/neco.1992.4.3.415

McLeod, 1996, Do fielders know where to go to catch the ball or only how to get there?, J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform., 22, 531, 10.1037/0096-1523.22.3.531

McNamara, 1985, Optimal foraging and learning, J. Theor. Biol., 117, 231, 10.1016/S0022-5193(85)80219-8

McNamara, 1987, Partial preferences and foraging, Anim. Behav., 35, 1084, 10.1016/S0003-3472(87)80166-5

McNamara, 1987, Memory and the efficient use of information, J. Theor. Biol., 125, 385, 10.1016/S0022-5193(87)80209-6

McNamara, 2004, Variation in behaviour promotes cooperation in the Prisoner's Dilemma game, Nature, 428, 745, 10.1038/nature02432

Mallon, 2000, Ants estimate area using Buffon's needle, Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B, 267, 765, 10.1098/rspb.2000.1069

Marchetti, 1998, The evolution of multiple male traits in the yellow-browed leaf warbler, Anim. Behav., 55, 361, 10.1006/anbe.1997.0586

Marr, 1982

Martignon, 2002, Fast, frugal, and fit: simple heuristics for paired comparisons, Theor. Decis., 52, 29, 10.1023/A:1015516217425

Martignon, 1999, Bayesian benchmarks for fast and frugal heuristics, 169

Maynard Smith, 1982

Menzel, 2001, Searching for the memory trace in a mini-brain, the honeybee, Learn. Mem., 8, 53, 10.1101/lm.38801

Menzel, 2001, Cognitive architecture of a mini-brain: the honeybee, Trends Cogn. Sci., 5, 62, 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01601-6

Menzel, 1993, Functional organization of appetitive learning and memory in a generalist pollinator the honey bee, 79

Moyer, 1967, Time required for judgements of numerical inequality, Nature, 215, 1519, 10.1038/2151519a0

Mugford, 2001, The accuracy of Buffon's needle: a rule of thumb used by ants to estimate area, Behav. Ecol., 12, 655, 10.1093/beheco/12.6.655

Müller, 1988, Path integration in desert ants, Cataglyphis fortis, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A., 85, 5287, 10.1073/pnas.85.14.5287

Nakata, 2003, Using past experience in web relocation decisions enhances the foraging efficiency of the spider Cyclosa argenteoalba, J. Insect Behav., 16, 371, 10.1023/A:1024828126119

Newell, 2003, Take the best or look at the rest? Factors influencing “one-reason” decision making, J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn., 29, 53, 10.1037/0278-7393.29.1.53

Oudejans, 1999, Shedding some light on catching in the dark: perceptual mechanisms for catching fly balls, J. Exp. Psych. Hum. Percept. Perform., 25, 531, 10.1037/0096-1523.25.2.531

Partan, 1999, Communication goes multimodal, Science, 283, 1272, 10.1126/science.283.5406.1272

Patricelli, 2003, Multiple male traits interact: attractive bower decorations facilitate attractive behavioural displays in satin bowerbirds, Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B, 270, 2389, 10.1098/rspb.2003.2530

Payne, 1993

Pratt, 2002, Quorum sensing, recruitment, and collective decision-making during colony emigration by the ant Leptothorax albipennis, Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 52, 117, 10.1007/s00265-002-0487-x

Proctor, 2001, Modelling antipredator vigilance and flight response in group foragers when warning signals are ambiguous, J. Theor. Biol., 211, 409, 10.1006/jtbi.2001.2353

Quinlan, 1993

Raguso, 2002, Synergy between visual and olfactory cues in nectar feeding by naïve hawkmoths, Manduca sexta, Anim. Behav., 64, 685, 10.1006/anbe.2002.4010

Real, 1990, Predator switching and the interpretation of animal choice behavior: the case for constrained optimization, 1

Real, 1990, Search theory and mate choice. I. Models of single-sex discrimination, Am. Nat., 136, 376, 10.1086/285103

Real, 1992, Information processing and the evolutionary ecology of cognitive architecture, Am. Nat., 140, S108, 10.1086/285399

Rechten, 1983, Optimal prey selection: why do great tits show partial preferences?, Anim. Behav., 31, 576, 10.1016/S0003-3472(83)80081-5

Reimer, 2004, The use of recognition in group decision-making, Cogn. Sci., 28, 1009, 10.1207/s15516709cog2806_6

Rieskamp, 1999, When do people use simple heuristics, and how can we tell?, 141

Rieskamp, J., Otto, P.E. Adaptive strategy selection in decision making: the learning rule theory, submitted for publication.

Roitberg, 1993, Choosing hosts and mates: the value of learning, 174

Römer, 2000, A gain-control mechanism for processing of chorus sounds in the afferent auditory pathway of the bushcricket Tettigonia viridissima (Orthoptera; Tettigoniidae), J. Comp. Physiol. A, 186, 181, 10.1007/s003590050018

Rosander, 2002, Development of gaze tracking of small and large objects, Exp. Brain Res., 146, 257, 10.1007/s00221-002-1161-2

Rowe, 1999, Receiver psychology and the evolution of multicomponent signals, Anim. Behav., 58, 921, 10.1006/anbe.1999.1242

Rowe, 2004, Avian psychology and communication, Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B, 271, 1435, 10.1098/rspb.2004.2753

Sato, 2003, Rules for nest sanitation of a social spider mite, Schizotetranychus miscanthi Saito (Acari: Tetranychidae), Ethology, 109, 713, 10.1046/j.1439-0310.2003.00905.x

Schuck-Paim, 2004, State-dependent decisions cause apparent violations of rationality in animal choice, PLoS Biol., 2, e402, 10.1371/journal.pbio.0020402

Seeley, 1995

Seeley, 2003, Consensus building during nest-site selection in honey bee swarms: the expiration of dissent, Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 53, 417, 10.1007/s00265-003-0598-z

Seeley, 2003, Choosing a home: how scouts in a honey bee swarm perceive the completion of their group decision making, Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 54, 511, 10.1007/s00265-003-0664-6

Seeley, 2004, Quorum sensing during nest-site selection by honeybee swarms, Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 56, 594, 10.1007/s00265-004-0814-5

Shaffer, 2004, How dogs navigate to catch frisbees, Psychol. Sci., 15, 437, 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00698.x

Shafir, 2002, Context-dependent violations of rational choice in honeybees (Apis mellifera) and gray jays (Perisoreus canadensis), Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 51, 180, 10.1007/s00265-001-0420-8

Shanteau, 2000, Fast and frugal heuristics: what about unfriendly environments?, Behav. Brain. Sci., 23, 762, 10.1017/S0140525X00463442

Shettleworth, 1998

Shine, 2003, The lexikon of love: what cues cause size-assortative courtship by male garter snakes, Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 53, 234, 10.1007/s00265-002-0568-x

Simão, 2002, Modeling mate choice in monogamous mating systems with courtship, Adapt. Behav., 10, 113, 10.1177/1059712302010002003

Simon, 1956, Rational choice and the structure of the environment, Psychol. Rev., 63, 129, 10.1037/h0042769

Simon, 1990, Invariants of human behavior, Ann. Rev. Psychol., 41, 1, 10.1146/annurev.ps.41.020190.000245

Simpson, 1999, When to switch to flowering, Ann. Rev. Cell. Dev. Biol., 99, 519, 10.1146/annurev.cellbio.15.1.519

Sirot, 2001, Mate-choice copying by females: the advantages of a prudent strategy, J. Evol. Biol., 14, 418, 10.1046/j.1420-9101.2001.00284.x

Stephens, 2002, Discrimination, discounting and impulsivity: a role for an informational constraint, Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B, 357, 1527, 10.1098/rstb.2002.1062

Stephens, 2001, The adaptive value of preference for immediacy: when shortsighted rules have farsighted consequences, Behav. Ecol., 12, 330, 10.1093/beheco/12.3.330

Tauber, 1976, Insect seasonality: diapause maintenance, termination, and postdiapause development, Ann. Rev. Entomol., 21, 81, 10.1146/annurev.en.21.010176.000501

Thompson, 1983, A comparative study of germination responses to diurnally-fluctuating temperatures, J. Appl. Ecol., 20, 141, 10.2307/2403382

Thomson, 1996, Trapline foraging by bumblebees: I. Persistence of flight-path geometry, Behav. Ecol., 7, 158, 10.1093/beheco/7.2.158

Thuijsman, 1995, Automata, matching and foraging behavior of bees, J. Theor. Biol., 175, 305, 10.1006/jtbi.1995.0144

Tinbergen, 1958

Todd, 1993, Psychological mechanisms and the Marginal Value Theorem: dynamics of scalar memory for travel time, Anim. Behav., 46, 765, 10.1006/anbe.1993.1254

Todd, 1999, Simple inference heuristics versus complex decision machines, Minds Mach., 9, 461, 10.1023/A:1008335515764

Todd, P.M., Dieckmann, A. Heuristics for ordering cue search in decision making. Adv. Neural Inform. Process. Syst. 18, in press.

Todd, 1999, From pride and prejudice to persuasion: satisficing in mate search, 287

Tversky, 1972, Elimination by aspects: a theory of choice, Psychol. Rev., 79, 281, 10.1037/h0032955

van Alphen, 2003, Information acquisition and time allocation in insect parasitoids, Trends. Ecol. Evol., 18, 81, 10.1016/S0169-5347(02)00035-6

Vicario, 2001, Sex differences in discrimination of vocal communication signals in a songbird, Anim. Behav., 61, 805, 10.1006/anbe.2000.1651

Vollrath, 1987, Altered geometry of webs in spiders with regenerated legs, Nature, 328, 247, 10.1038/328247a0

Volz, K.G., Schubotz, R.I., Raab, M., Schooler, L., Gigerenzer, G., von Cramon, D.Y. Recognition heuristic and the brain. EPOS/NOW workshop: control in attention and action: neurocognitive systems and mechanisms. Amsterdam, in press.

Wajnberg, 2000, Patch leaving decision rules and the Marginal Value Theorem: an experimental analysis and a simulation model, Behav. Ecol., 11, 577, 10.1093/beheco/11.6.577

Wajnberg, 2003, A comparative analysis of patch-leaving decision rules in a parasitoid family, J. Anim. Ecol., 72, 618, 10.1046/j.1365-2656.2003.00731.x

Wald, 1947

Wallraff, 2001, Navigation by homing pigeons: updated perspective, Ethol. Ecol. Evol., 13, 1, 10.1080/08927014.2001.9522786

Webb, 2003, Reafferent or redundant: integration of phonotaxis and optomotor behavior in crickets and robots, Adapt. Behav., 11, 137, 10.1177/1059712303113001

Wells, 1988, Effects of body size and resource value on fighting behaviour in a jumping spider, Anim. Behav., 36, 321, 10.1016/S0003-3472(88)80001-0

Wiegmann, 1998, The fixed sample search rule and use of an indicator character to evaluate mate quality, J. Theor. Biol., 193, 709, 10.1006/jtbi.1998.0737

Wiegmann, 1996, Some distinguishing features of models of search behavior and mate choice, Am. Nat., 147, 188, 10.1086/285846

Wilke, A., Hutchinson, J.M.C., Todd, P.M., 2004. Testing simple rules for human foraging in patchy environments. In: Forbus, K., Gentner, D., Regier, T. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 26th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ, p. 1656.

Williams, 1956, Physiology of insect diapause. X. An endocrine mechanism for the influence of temperature on the diapausing pupa of the cecropia silkworm, Biol. Bull., 110, 201, 10.2307/1538982

Wiltschko, 2003, Avian navigation: from historical to modern concepts, Anim. Behav., 65, 257, 10.1006/anbe.2003.2054

Ydenberg, 1982, Great tits and giving-up times: decision rules for leaving patches, Behaviour, 90, 1, 10.1163/156853984X00533

Zuk, 1992, Effects of experimental manipulation of male secondary sex characters on female mate preference in red jungle fowl, Anim. Behav., 44, 999, 10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80312-4