Simple heuristics and rules of thumb: Where psychologists and behavioural biologists might meet
Tóm tắt
Từ khóa
Tài liệu tham khảo
Anderson, 1990
Arkes, 1999, The sunk cost and concorde effects: are humans less rational than lower animals?, Psych. Bull., 125, 591, 10.1037/0033-2909.125.5.591
Balda, 1992, Long-term spatial memory in Clark's nutcracker, Nucifruga columbiana, Anim. Behav., 44, 761, 10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80302-1
Balkovsky, 2002, Olfactory search at high Reynolds number, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A., 99, 12589, 10.1073/pnas.192393499
Barta, 2002, Sexual conflict about parental care: the role of reserves, Am. Nat., 159, 687, 10.1086/339995
Basolo, 2002, The confirmation of a female preference for a composite male trait in green swordtails, Anim. Behav., 63, 469, 10.1006/anbe.2001.1933
Bateson, 2003, Context-dependent foraging decisions in rufous hummingbirds, Proc. R. Soc. Lond., 270, 1271, 10.1098/rspb.2003.2365
Baum, 2001, Hummingbird foraging behavior in different patch types: simulation of alternative strategies, Ecol. Mod., 137, 201, 10.1016/S0304-3800(00)00436-1
Bélisle, 1997, The effects of a limited memory capacity on foraging behavior, Theor. Pop. Biol., 52, 78, 10.1006/tpbi.1997.1319
Benhamou, 1992, Efficiency of area-concentrated searching behaviour in a continuous patchy environment, J. Theor. Biol., 159, 67, 10.1016/S0022-5193(05)80768-4
Bonduriansky, 2003, Layered sexual selection: a comparative analysis of sexual behaviour within an assemblage of piophilid flies, Can. J. Zool., 81, 479, 10.1139/z03-031
Bookstaber, 1985, On the optimality of coarse behavior rules, J. Theor. Biol., 116, 161, 10.1016/S0022-5193(85)80262-9
Bradbeer, 1988
Brancazio, 1985, Looking into Chapman's homer: the physics of judging a fly ball, Am. J. Phys., 53, 849, 10.1119/1.14350
Breiman, 1984
Brewer, 1996
Bröder, 2000, Assessing the empirical validity of the “Take-the-Best” heuristic as a model of human probabilistic inference, J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn., 26, 1332, 10.1037/0278-7393.26.5.1332
Bröder, 2003, Decision making with the “adaptive toolbox”: influence of environmental structure, intelligence, and working memory load, J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn., 29, 611, 10.1037/0278-7393.29.4.611
Bröder, 2003, Take The Best versus simultaneous feature matching: probabilistic inferences form memory and effects of representation format, J. Exp. Psychol. Gen., 132, 277, 10.1037/0096-3445.132.2.277
Buntine, 1992
Burke, 2003, An evolved spatial memory bias in a nectar-feeding bird?, Anim. Behav., 66, 695, 10.1006/anbe.2003.2246
Calkins, 2003, Mate choice for multiple ornaments in the California quail, Callipepla californica, Anim. Behav., 65, 69, 10.1006/anbe.2002.2041
Camazine, 2001
Candolin, 2003, The use of multiple cues in mate choice, Biol. Rev., 78, 575, 10.1017/S1464793103006158
Charnov, 1976, Optimal foraging, the marginal value theorem, Theor. Pop. Biol., 9, 129, 10.1016/0040-5809(76)90040-X
Chater, 1999, Ten years of the rational analysis of cognition, Trends Cogn. Sci., 3, 57, 10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01273-X
Chater, 2003, Fast, frugal, and rational: how rational norms explain behavior, Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process., 90, 63, 10.1016/S0749-5978(02)00508-3
Cheverton, 1985, Optimal foraging: constraints and currencies, 109
Cresswell, 2000, Evidence for a rule governing the avoidance of superfluous escape flights, Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B, 267, 733, 10.1098/rspb.2000.1064
Czerlinski, 1999, How good are simple heuristics?, 97
Davies, 1981, Owners and satellites: the economics of territory defence in the pied wagtail, Motacilla alba, J. Anim. Ecol., 50, 157, 10.2307/4038
Davis, 1999, Environment quality predicts parental provisioning decisions, Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B, 266, 1791, 10.1098/rspb.1999.0848
Dawes, 1979, The robust beauty of improper linear models in decision making, Am. Psychol., 34, 571, 10.1037/0003-066X.34.7.571
Dawkins, 1982
Detrain, 2002, Complexity of environment and parsimony of decision rules in insect societies, Biol. Bull., 202, 268, 10.2307/1543478
de Vries, 2002, Self-organization in collective honeybee foraging: emergence of symmetry breaking, cross inhibition and equal harvest-rate distribution, Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 51, 557, 10.1007/s00265-002-0454-6
Dhami, 2003, Psychological models of professional decision making, Psychol. Sci., 14, 175, 10.1111/1467-9280.01438
Dieckmann, A., Todd, P.M., 2004. Simple ways to construct search orders. In: Forbus, K., Gentner, D., Regier, T. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 26th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ, pp. 309–314.
Driessen, 1999, Patch departure mechanisms and optimal host exploitation in an insect parasitoid, J. Anim. Ecol., 68, 445, 10.1046/j.1365-2656.1999.00296.x
1998
Dusenbery, 2001, Performance of basic strategies for following gradients in two dimensions, J. Theor. Biol., 208, 345, 10.1006/jtbi.2000.2224
Enquist, 1990, A test of the sequential assessment game: fighting in the cichlid fish Nannacara anomala, Anim. Behav., 10, 1, 10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80660-8
Fairhurst, 2003, Temporal landmarks: proximity prevails, Anim. Cogn., 6, 113, 10.1007/s10071-003-0169-8
Fantino, 1998, Judgement and decision making: behavioral approaches, Behav. Anal., 21, 203, 10.1007/BF03391964
Fantino, 1985, Choice, optimal foraging, and the delay-reduction hypothesis, Behav. Brain. Sci., 8, 315, 10.1017/S0140525X00020847
Fawcett, T.W., 2003. Multiple cues and variation in mate-choice behaviour. D.Phil. Thesis. University of Cambridge.
Fawcett, 2003, Optimal assessment of multiple cues, Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B, 270, 1637, 10.1098/rspb.2003.2328
Fortin, 2003, Searching behavior and use of sampling information by free-ranging bison (Bos bison), Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 54, 194, 10.1007/s00265-003-0599-y
Fraenkel, 1940
Franks, 2003, Strategies for choosing between alternatives with different attributes: exemplified by house-hunting ants, Anim. Behav., 65, 215, 10.1006/anbe.2002.2032
Gibson, 1996, Female choice in sage grouse: the roles of attraction and active comparison, Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 39, 55, 10.1007/s002650050266
Gigerenzer, 2000
Gigerenzer, 2004, Fast and frugal heuristics: the tools of bounded rationality, 62
Gigerenzer, 1996, Reasoning the fast and frugal way: models of bounded rationality, Psychol. Rev., 103, 650, 10.1037/0033-295X.103.4.650
Gigerenzer, 1999, Betting on one good reason: the Take The Best heuristic, 75
2001
Gigerenzer, 1999
Gill, 1988, Trapline foraging by hermit hummingbirds: competition for an undefended, renewable resource, Ecology, 69, 1933, 10.2307/1941170
2002
Goldstein, 2002, Models of ecological rationality: the recognition heuristic, Psychol. Rev., 109, 75, 10.1037/0033-295X.109.1.75
Gould, 1988
Gould, 1991
Gould, 1979, The spandrels of San Marco and the Panglossian paradigm: a critique of the adaptationist programme, Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B, 205, 581, 10.1098/rspb.1979.0086
Goulson, 2000, Why do pollinators visit proportionally fewer flowers in large patches?, Oikos, 91, 485, 10.1034/j.1600-0706.2000.910309.x
Grafen, 1990, Biological signals as handicaps, J. Theor. Biol., 144, 517, 10.1016/S0022-5193(05)80088-8
Hammerstein, 2003, Why is reciprocity so rare in social animals? A protestant appeal, 83
Hankinson, 2003, Avoiding a compromise between sexual selection and species recognition: female swordtail fish assess multiple species-specific cues, Behav. Ecol., 14, 282, 10.1093/beheco/14.2.282
Hasson, 1991, Sexual displays as amplifiers: practical examples with an emphasis on feather decorations, Behav. Ecol., 2, 189, 10.1093/beheco/2.3.189
Hauert, 2002, Simple adaptive strategy wins the prisoner's dilemma, J. Theor. Biol., 218, 261, 10.1006/jtbi.2002.3072
Hebets, 2005, Attention-altering signal interactions in the multimodal courtship display of the wolf spider Schizocosa uetzi, Behav. Ecol., 16, 75, 10.1093/beheco/arh133
Henrich, 1998, The evolution of conformist transmission and the emergence of between-group differences, Evol. Hum. Behav., 19, 215, 10.1016/S1090-5138(98)00018-X
Hertwig, 1999, Quick estimation: letting the environment do the work, 209
Hertwig, 2003, More is not always better: the benefits of cognitive limits, 213
Hill, 1999, Mate choice based on static versus dynamic secondary sexual traits in the dark-eyed junco, Behav. Ecol., 10, 91, 10.1093/beheco/10.1.91
Hills, 2002, Time's crooked arrow: optimal foraging and rate-biased time perception, Anim. Behav., 64, 589, 10.1006/anbe.2002.3070
Hoffmann, 2000, Twenty years on: the evolution of cooperation revisited, J. Artif. Soc. Soc. Simul., 3
Hoffrage, 2000, Hindsight bias: a by-product of knowledge updating?, J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn., 26, 566, 10.1037/0278-7393.26.3.566
Houston, 1982, Some learning rules for acquiring information, 140
Hutchinson, 2004, Mate choice when males are in patches: optimal strategies and good rules of thumb, J. Theor. Biol., 231, 129, 10.1016/j.jtbi.2004.06.009
Hutchinson, 1993, Song, sexual selection, starvation and strategic handicaps, Anim. Behav., 45, 1153, 10.1006/anbe.1993.1139
Iwasa, 1981, Prey distribution as a factor determining the choice of optimal foraging strategy, Am. Nat., 117, 710, 10.1086/283754
Janetos, 1980, Strategies of female mate choice: a theoretical analysis, Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 7, 107, 10.1007/BF00299515
Jennions, 1997, Variation in mate choice and mating preferences: a review of causes and consequences, Biol. Rev., 72, 283, 10.1017/S0006323196005014
Johnson, 1989, When choice models fail: compensatory models in negatively correlated environments, J. Market. Res., 26, 255, 10.2307/3172899
Kacelnik, 1992, Psychological mechanisms and the Marginal Value Theorem: effect of variability in travel time on patch exploitation, Anim. Behav., 43, 313, 10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80226-X
Kahneman, 1979, Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under risk, Econometrica, 47, 263, 10.2307/1914185
Kahneman, 1996, On the reality of cognitive illusions, Psychol. Rev., 103, 582, 10.1037/0033-295X.103.3.582
Kamil, 1981
Kareev, 2000, Seven (indeed, plus or minus two) and the detection of correlations, Psychol. Rev., 107, 397, 10.1037/0033-295X.107.2.397
Karsai, 2000, Optimality of cell arrangement and rules of thumb of cell initiation in Polistes dominulus: a modeling approach, Behav. Ecol., 11, 387, 10.1093/beheco/11.4.387
Keasar, 2002, Bees in two-armed bandit situations: foraging choices and possible decision mechanisms, Behav. Ecol., 13, 757, 10.1093/beheco/13.6.757
Kennedy, 1983, Zigzagging and casting as a programmed response to wind-borne odour: a review, Physiol. Entomol., 8, 109, 10.1111/j.1365-3032.1983.tb00340.x
Kodric-Brown, 2001, Female choice in the guppy (Poecilia reticulata): the interaction between male color and display, Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 50, 346, 10.1007/s002650100374
Krebs, 1977, Optimal prey selection in the great tit (Parus major), Anim. Behav., 25, 30, 10.1016/0003-3472(77)90064-1
Krebs, 1983, Perspectives in optimal foraging, 165
Künzler, 2001, Female preferences for single and combined traits in computer animated stickleback males, Behav. Ecol., 12, 681, 10.1093/beheco/12.6.681
1985
Lifjeld, 1988, Female pied flycatchers Ficedula hypoleuca choose male characteristics in homogeneous habitats, Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 22, 27, 10.1007/BF00395695
Lima, 1994, Collective detection of predatory attack by birds in the absence of alarm signals, J. Avian Biol., 25, 319, 10.2307/3677279
Logue, 1988, Research on self-control: an integrating framework, Behav. Brain. Sci., 11, 665, 10.1017/S0140525X00053978
Luttbeg, 1996, A comparative Bayes tactic for mate assessment and choice, Behav. Ecol., 7, 451, 10.1093/beheco/7.4.451
McLeod, 1996, Do fielders know where to go to catch the ball or only how to get there?, J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform., 22, 531, 10.1037/0096-1523.22.3.531
McNamara, 1985, Optimal foraging and learning, J. Theor. Biol., 117, 231, 10.1016/S0022-5193(85)80219-8
McNamara, 1987, Partial preferences and foraging, Anim. Behav., 35, 1084, 10.1016/S0003-3472(87)80166-5
McNamara, 1987, Memory and the efficient use of information, J. Theor. Biol., 125, 385, 10.1016/S0022-5193(87)80209-6
McNamara, 2004, Variation in behaviour promotes cooperation in the Prisoner's Dilemma game, Nature, 428, 745, 10.1038/nature02432
Mallon, 2000, Ants estimate area using Buffon's needle, Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B, 267, 765, 10.1098/rspb.2000.1069
Marchetti, 1998, The evolution of multiple male traits in the yellow-browed leaf warbler, Anim. Behav., 55, 361, 10.1006/anbe.1997.0586
Marr, 1982
Martignon, 2002, Fast, frugal, and fit: simple heuristics for paired comparisons, Theor. Decis., 52, 29, 10.1023/A:1015516217425
Martignon, 1999, Bayesian benchmarks for fast and frugal heuristics, 169
Maynard Smith, 1982
Menzel, 2001, Searching for the memory trace in a mini-brain, the honeybee, Learn. Mem., 8, 53, 10.1101/lm.38801
Menzel, 2001, Cognitive architecture of a mini-brain: the honeybee, Trends Cogn. Sci., 5, 62, 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01601-6
Menzel, 1993, Functional organization of appetitive learning and memory in a generalist pollinator the honey bee, 79
Moyer, 1967, Time required for judgements of numerical inequality, Nature, 215, 1519, 10.1038/2151519a0
Mugford, 2001, The accuracy of Buffon's needle: a rule of thumb used by ants to estimate area, Behav. Ecol., 12, 655, 10.1093/beheco/12.6.655
Müller, 1988, Path integration in desert ants, Cataglyphis fortis, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A., 85, 5287, 10.1073/pnas.85.14.5287
Nakata, 2003, Using past experience in web relocation decisions enhances the foraging efficiency of the spider Cyclosa argenteoalba, J. Insect Behav., 16, 371, 10.1023/A:1024828126119
Newell, 2003, Take the best or look at the rest? Factors influencing “one-reason” decision making, J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn., 29, 53, 10.1037/0278-7393.29.1.53
Oudejans, 1999, Shedding some light on catching in the dark: perceptual mechanisms for catching fly balls, J. Exp. Psych. Hum. Percept. Perform., 25, 531, 10.1037/0096-1523.25.2.531
Patricelli, 2003, Multiple male traits interact: attractive bower decorations facilitate attractive behavioural displays in satin bowerbirds, Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B, 270, 2389, 10.1098/rspb.2003.2530
Payne, 1993
Pratt, 2002, Quorum sensing, recruitment, and collective decision-making during colony emigration by the ant Leptothorax albipennis, Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 52, 117, 10.1007/s00265-002-0487-x
Proctor, 2001, Modelling antipredator vigilance and flight response in group foragers when warning signals are ambiguous, J. Theor. Biol., 211, 409, 10.1006/jtbi.2001.2353
Quinlan, 1993
Raguso, 2002, Synergy between visual and olfactory cues in nectar feeding by naïve hawkmoths, Manduca sexta, Anim. Behav., 64, 685, 10.1006/anbe.2002.4010
Real, 1990, Predator switching and the interpretation of animal choice behavior: the case for constrained optimization, 1
Real, 1990, Search theory and mate choice. I. Models of single-sex discrimination, Am. Nat., 136, 376, 10.1086/285103
Real, 1992, Information processing and the evolutionary ecology of cognitive architecture, Am. Nat., 140, S108, 10.1086/285399
Rechten, 1983, Optimal prey selection: why do great tits show partial preferences?, Anim. Behav., 31, 576, 10.1016/S0003-3472(83)80081-5
Reimer, 2004, The use of recognition in group decision-making, Cogn. Sci., 28, 1009, 10.1207/s15516709cog2806_6
Rieskamp, 1999, When do people use simple heuristics, and how can we tell?, 141
Rieskamp, J., Otto, P.E. Adaptive strategy selection in decision making: the learning rule theory, submitted for publication.
Roitberg, 1993, Choosing hosts and mates: the value of learning, 174
Römer, 2000, A gain-control mechanism for processing of chorus sounds in the afferent auditory pathway of the bushcricket Tettigonia viridissima (Orthoptera; Tettigoniidae), J. Comp. Physiol. A, 186, 181, 10.1007/s003590050018
Rosander, 2002, Development of gaze tracking of small and large objects, Exp. Brain Res., 146, 257, 10.1007/s00221-002-1161-2
Rowe, 1999, Receiver psychology and the evolution of multicomponent signals, Anim. Behav., 58, 921, 10.1006/anbe.1999.1242
Rowe, 2004, Avian psychology and communication, Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B, 271, 1435, 10.1098/rspb.2004.2753
Sato, 2003, Rules for nest sanitation of a social spider mite, Schizotetranychus miscanthi Saito (Acari: Tetranychidae), Ethology, 109, 713, 10.1046/j.1439-0310.2003.00905.x
Schuck-Paim, 2004, State-dependent decisions cause apparent violations of rationality in animal choice, PLoS Biol., 2, e402, 10.1371/journal.pbio.0020402
Seeley, 1995
Seeley, 2003, Consensus building during nest-site selection in honey bee swarms: the expiration of dissent, Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 53, 417, 10.1007/s00265-003-0598-z
Seeley, 2003, Choosing a home: how scouts in a honey bee swarm perceive the completion of their group decision making, Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 54, 511, 10.1007/s00265-003-0664-6
Seeley, 2004, Quorum sensing during nest-site selection by honeybee swarms, Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 56, 594, 10.1007/s00265-004-0814-5
Shaffer, 2004, How dogs navigate to catch frisbees, Psychol. Sci., 15, 437, 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00698.x
Shafir, 2002, Context-dependent violations of rational choice in honeybees (Apis mellifera) and gray jays (Perisoreus canadensis), Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 51, 180, 10.1007/s00265-001-0420-8
Shanteau, 2000, Fast and frugal heuristics: what about unfriendly environments?, Behav. Brain. Sci., 23, 762, 10.1017/S0140525X00463442
Shettleworth, 1998
Shine, 2003, The lexikon of love: what cues cause size-assortative courtship by male garter snakes, Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 53, 234, 10.1007/s00265-002-0568-x
Simão, 2002, Modeling mate choice in monogamous mating systems with courtship, Adapt. Behav., 10, 113, 10.1177/1059712302010002003
Simon, 1956, Rational choice and the structure of the environment, Psychol. Rev., 63, 129, 10.1037/h0042769
Simon, 1990, Invariants of human behavior, Ann. Rev. Psychol., 41, 1, 10.1146/annurev.ps.41.020190.000245
Simpson, 1999, When to switch to flowering, Ann. Rev. Cell. Dev. Biol., 99, 519, 10.1146/annurev.cellbio.15.1.519
Sirot, 2001, Mate-choice copying by females: the advantages of a prudent strategy, J. Evol. Biol., 14, 418, 10.1046/j.1420-9101.2001.00284.x
Stephens, 2002, Discrimination, discounting and impulsivity: a role for an informational constraint, Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B, 357, 1527, 10.1098/rstb.2002.1062
Stephens, 2001, The adaptive value of preference for immediacy: when shortsighted rules have farsighted consequences, Behav. Ecol., 12, 330, 10.1093/beheco/12.3.330
Tauber, 1976, Insect seasonality: diapause maintenance, termination, and postdiapause development, Ann. Rev. Entomol., 21, 81, 10.1146/annurev.en.21.010176.000501
Thompson, 1983, A comparative study of germination responses to diurnally-fluctuating temperatures, J. Appl. Ecol., 20, 141, 10.2307/2403382
Thomson, 1996, Trapline foraging by bumblebees: I. Persistence of flight-path geometry, Behav. Ecol., 7, 158, 10.1093/beheco/7.2.158
Thuijsman, 1995, Automata, matching and foraging behavior of bees, J. Theor. Biol., 175, 305, 10.1006/jtbi.1995.0144
Tinbergen, 1958
Todd, 1993, Psychological mechanisms and the Marginal Value Theorem: dynamics of scalar memory for travel time, Anim. Behav., 46, 765, 10.1006/anbe.1993.1254
Todd, 1999, Simple inference heuristics versus complex decision machines, Minds Mach., 9, 461, 10.1023/A:1008335515764
Todd, P.M., Dieckmann, A. Heuristics for ordering cue search in decision making. Adv. Neural Inform. Process. Syst. 18, in press.
Todd, 1999, From pride and prejudice to persuasion: satisficing in mate search, 287
van Alphen, 2003, Information acquisition and time allocation in insect parasitoids, Trends. Ecol. Evol., 18, 81, 10.1016/S0169-5347(02)00035-6
Vicario, 2001, Sex differences in discrimination of vocal communication signals in a songbird, Anim. Behav., 61, 805, 10.1006/anbe.2000.1651
Vollrath, 1987, Altered geometry of webs in spiders with regenerated legs, Nature, 328, 247, 10.1038/328247a0
Volz, K.G., Schubotz, R.I., Raab, M., Schooler, L., Gigerenzer, G., von Cramon, D.Y. Recognition heuristic and the brain. EPOS/NOW workshop: control in attention and action: neurocognitive systems and mechanisms. Amsterdam, in press.
Wajnberg, 2000, Patch leaving decision rules and the Marginal Value Theorem: an experimental analysis and a simulation model, Behav. Ecol., 11, 577, 10.1093/beheco/11.6.577
Wajnberg, 2003, A comparative analysis of patch-leaving decision rules in a parasitoid family, J. Anim. Ecol., 72, 618, 10.1046/j.1365-2656.2003.00731.x
Wald, 1947
Wallraff, 2001, Navigation by homing pigeons: updated perspective, Ethol. Ecol. Evol., 13, 1, 10.1080/08927014.2001.9522786
Webb, 2003, Reafferent or redundant: integration of phonotaxis and optomotor behavior in crickets and robots, Adapt. Behav., 11, 137, 10.1177/1059712303113001
Wells, 1988, Effects of body size and resource value on fighting behaviour in a jumping spider, Anim. Behav., 36, 321, 10.1016/S0003-3472(88)80001-0
Wiegmann, 1998, The fixed sample search rule and use of an indicator character to evaluate mate quality, J. Theor. Biol., 193, 709, 10.1006/jtbi.1998.0737
Wiegmann, 1996, Some distinguishing features of models of search behavior and mate choice, Am. Nat., 147, 188, 10.1086/285846
Wilke, A., Hutchinson, J.M.C., Todd, P.M., 2004. Testing simple rules for human foraging in patchy environments. In: Forbus, K., Gentner, D., Regier, T. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 26th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ, p. 1656.
Williams, 1956, Physiology of insect diapause. X. An endocrine mechanism for the influence of temperature on the diapausing pupa of the cecropia silkworm, Biol. Bull., 110, 201, 10.2307/1538982
Wiltschko, 2003, Avian navigation: from historical to modern concepts, Anim. Behav., 65, 257, 10.1006/anbe.2003.2054
Ydenberg, 1982, Great tits and giving-up times: decision rules for leaving patches, Behaviour, 90, 1, 10.1163/156853984X00533