Semantic interference from distractor pictures in single-picture naming: evidence for competitive lexical selection

Jörg D. Jescheniak1, Asya Matushanskaya1, Andreas Mädebach1, Matthias M. Müller1
1Department of Psychology, University of Leipzig, Leipzig, Germany

Tóm tắt

Picture-naming studies have demonstrated interference from semantic-categorically related distractor words, but not from corresponding distractor pictures, and the lack of generality of the interference effect has been argued to challenge theories viewing lexical selection in speech production as a competitive process. Here, we demonstrate that semantic interference from context pictures does become visible, if sufficient attention is allocated to them. We combined picture naming with a spatial-cuing procedure. When participants’ attention was shifted to the distractor, semantically related distractor pictures interfered with the response, as compared with unrelated distractor pictures. This finding supports models conceiving lexical retrieval as competitive (Levelt, Roelofs, & Meyer, 1999) but is difficult to reconcile with the response exclusion hypothesis (Finkbeiner & Caramazza, 2006b) proposed as an alternative.

Từ khóa


Tài liệu tham khảo

Abdel Rahman, R., & Melinger, A. (2009). Semantic context effects in language production: A swinging lexical network proposal and a review. Language and Cognitive Processes, 24, 713–734. doi:10.1080/01690960802597250 Andersen, S. K., Fuchs, S., & Müller, M. M. (2011). Effects of feature-selective and spatial attention at different stages of visual processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 238–246. doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21328 Aristei, S., Zwitserlood, P., & Rahman, R. A. (2012). Picture-induced semantic interference reflects lexical competition during object naming. Frontiers in Psychology, 3. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00028 Bloem, I., & La Heij, W. (2003). Semantic facilitation and semantic interference in word translation: Implications for models of lexical access in language production. Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 468–488. doi:10.1016/S0749-596X(02)00503-X Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A speading-activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82, 407–428. doi:10.1037//0033-295X.82.6.407 Damian, M. F., & Bowers, J. S. (2003). Locus of semantic interference in picture-word interference tasks. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10, 111–117. doi:10.3758/BF03196474 Damian, M. E., & Martin, R. C. (1999). Semantic and phonological codes interact in single word production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 345–361. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.25.2.345 De Jong, R., Berendsen, E., & Cools, R. (1999). Goal neglect and inhibitory limitations: Dissociable causes of interference effects in conflict situations. Acta Psychologica, 101, 379–394. doi:10.1016/S0001-6918(99)00012-8 Finkbeiner, M., & Caramazza, A. (2006a). Lexical selection is not a competitive process: A reply to La Heij et al. (2006). Cortex, 42, 1032–1035. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70210-7 Finkbeiner, M., & Caramazza, A. (2006b). Now you see it, now you don't: On turning semantic interference into facilitation in a Stroop-like task. Cortex, 42, 790–796. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70419-2 Glaser, W. R., & Düngelhoff, F.-J. (1984). The time course of picture-word interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 640–654. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.10.5.640 Glaser, W. R., & Glaser, M. O. (1989). Context effects in Stroop-like word and picture processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118, 13–42. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.118.1.13 Grison, S., Tipper, S. P., & Hewitt, O. (2005). Long-term negative priming: Support for retrieval of prior attentional processes. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 58, 1199–1224. doi:10.1080/02724980443000557 Hantsch, A., Jescheniak, J. D., & Schriefers, H. (2009). Distractor modality can turn semantic interference into semantic facilitation in the picture–word interference task: Implications for theories of lexical access in speech production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 1443–1453. doi:10.1037/a0017020 Humphreys, G. W., Lloyd-Jones, T. J., & Fias, W. (1995). Semantic interference on naming using a post-cue procedure: Tapping the links between semantics and phonology with pictures and words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 961–980. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.21.4.961 Jescheniak, J. D., Oppermann, F., Hantsch, A., Wagner, V., Mädebach, A., & Schriefers, H. (2009). Do perceived context pictures automatically activate their phonological code? Experimental Psychology, 56, 56–65. doi:10.1027/1618-3169.56.1.56 Jescheniak, J. D., Schriefers, H., & Hantsch, A. (2001). Semantic and phonological activation in noun and pronoun production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 1058–1078. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.27.4.1058 Kuipers, J.-R., & La Heij, W. (2009). The limitations of cascading in the speech production system. Language and Cognitive Processes, 24, 120–135. doi:10.1080/01690960802234177 Kuipers, J. R., La Heij, W., & Costa, A. (2006). A further look at semantic context effects in language production: The role of response congruency. Language and Cognitive Processes, 21, 892–919. doi:10.1080/016909600824211 La Heij, W., Boelens, H., & Kuipers, J.-R. (2010). Object interference in children's colour and position naming: Lexical interference or task-set competition? Language and Cognitive Processes, 25, 568–588. doi:10.1080/01690960903381174 La Heij, W., Heikoop, K. W., Akerboom, S., & Bloem, I. (2003). Picture naming in picture context: Semantic interference or semantic facilitation? Psychology Science, 45, 49–62. Levelt, W. J. M. (1999). Models of word production. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 223–232. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01319-4 Levelt, W. J. M., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 1–75. doi:10.1017/S0140525X99001776 Mädebach, A., Jescheniak, J. D., Oppermann, F., & Schriefers, H. (2011). Ease of processing constrains the activation flow in the conceptual-lexical system during speech planning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37, 649–660. doi:10.1037/a0022330 Mahon, B. Z., Costa, A., Peterson, R., Vargas, K. A., & Caramazza, A. (2007). Lexical selection is not by competition: A reinterpretation of semantic interference and facilitation effects in the picture-word interference paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 503–535. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.33.3.503 Meyer, A. S., & Damian, M. F. (2007). Activation of distractor names in the picture-picture interference paradigm. Memory and Cognition, 35, 494–503. doi:10.3758/BF03193289 Morsella, E., & Miozzo, M. (2002). Evidence for a cascade model of lexical access in speech production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28, 555–563. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.28.3.555 Navarrete, E., & Costa, A. (2005). Phonological activation of ignored pictures: Further evidence for a cascade model of lexical access. Journal of Memory and Language, 53, 359–377. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2005.05.001 Oppermann, F., Jescheniak, J. D., & Schriefers, H. (2008). Conceptual coherence affects phonological activation of context objects during object naming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 587–601. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.34.3.587 Piai, V., Roelofs, A., & Schriefers, H. (2012). Distractor strength and selective attention in picture-naming performance. Memory & Cognition, 40, 614–627. doi:10.3758/s13421-011-0171-3 Posner, M. I. (1978). Chronometric explorations of mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 3–25. doi:10.1080/00335558008248231 Posner, M. I., Snyder, C. R., & Davidson, B. J. (1980). Attention and the detection of signals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109, 160–174. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.109.2.160 Ratcliff, R. (1979). Group reaction time distributions and an analysis of distribution statistics. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 446–461. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.86.3.446 Roelofs, A. (1992). A spreading-activation theory of lemma retrieval in speaking. Cognition, 42, 107–142. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(92)90041-F Roelofs, A. (2003). Goal-referenced selection of verbal action: Modeling attentional control in the Stroop task. Psychological Review, 110, 88–125. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.110.1.88 Roelofs, A. (2008a). Dynamics of the attentional control of word retrieval: Analyses of response time distributions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137, 303–323. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.137.2.303 Roelofs, A. (2008b). Tracing attention and the activation flow in spoken word planning using eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 353–368. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.34.2.353 Schriefers, H., Meyer, A. S., & Levelt, W. J. (1990). Exploring the time course of lexical access in language production - Picture-word interference studies. Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 86–102. doi:10.1016/0749-596X(90)90011-N Starreveld, P. A., & La Heij, W. (1995). Semantic interference, orthographic facilitation, and their interaction in naming tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 686–698. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.21.3.686