Rumination and Age: Some Things Get Better

Journal of Aging Research - Tập 2012 - Trang 1-10 - 2012
Stefan Sütterlin1,2, Muirne C. S. Paap3, Stana Babic4, Andrea Kübler4, Claus Vögele2
1Research Group on Health Psychology, University of Leuven, Tiensestraat 102, 3000 Leuven, Belgium
2Research Unit INSIDE, University of Luxembourg, Campus Walferdange, Route de Diekirch, 7220 Walferdange, Luxembourg
3Department of Research Methodology, Measurement and Data Analysis, Faculty of Behavioral Sciences, University of Twente, Drienerlolaan 5, 7522 NB Enschede, The Netherlands
4Department of Psychology I, University of Würzburg, Marcusstraße 9-11, 97070 Würzburg, Germany

Tóm tắt

Rumination has been defined as a mode of responding to distress that involves passively focusing one's attention on symptoms of distress without taking action. This dysfunctional response style intensifies depressed mood, impairs interpersonal problem solving, and leads to more pessimistic future perspectives and less social support. As most of these results were obtained from younger people, it remains unclear how age affects ruminative thinking. Three hundred members of the general public ranging in age from 15 to 87 years were asked about their ruminative styles using the Response Styles Questionnaire (RSQ), depression and satisfaction with life. A Mokken Scale analysis confirmed the two-factor structure of the RSQ with brooding and reflective pondering as subcomponents of rumination. Older participants (63 years and older) reported less ruminative thinking than other age groups. Life satisfaction was associated with brooding and highest for the earlier and latest life stages investigated in this study.

Từ khóa


Tài liệu tham khảo

10.1037/0022-3514.85.5.909

10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.163

10.1016/j.jpsychores.2005.06.074

10.1037/0021-843X.100.4.569

1987, Psychological Bulletin, 101, 259, 10.1037/0033-2909.101.2.259

1991, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 115, 10.1037/0022-3514.61.1.115

2008, Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 400, 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00088.x

10.1037//0021-843X.106.2.221

2000, Clinical Psychology, 7, 224

1995, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 176, 10.1037/0022-3514.69.1.176

10.1016/S0005-7967(01)00098-5

10.1016/j.brat.2007.07.015

10.1037/0022-3514.77.4.801

10.1002/jclp.20647

10.1037/1528-3542.1.1.25

1976

1985, Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 71, 10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13

1993, Psychological Assessment, 5, 164, 10.1037/1040-3590.5.2.164

1991, Personality and Individual Differences, 12, 117, 10.1016/0191-8869(91)90094-R

1989, Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 21, 210, 10.1037/h0079854

10.1023/A:1023910315561

10.1080/13607860701529619

10.1177/1073191109340388

10.1016/j.amepre.2006.07.007

10.1017/S0033291799001452

10.1017/S0033291705004976

10.1196/annals.1440.010

10.1196/annals.1308.001

1951, The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 78, 175

10.1186/1477-7525-2-40

10.1037//0882-7974.7.2.171

2003, Journals of Gerontology Series A, 58, 249, 10.1093/gerona/58.3.M249

2006, Mental health among the baby boomers

1987, Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 21, 46, 10.3109/00048678709160898

1984, Archives of General Psychiatry, 41, 949, 10.1001/archpsyc.1984.01790210031005

10.1097/01.JGP.0000235763.50230.83

1993

10.1177/014662167700100306

10.1111/j.1743-6109.2010.02066.x

2002, 5

2008, Neuro Endocrinology Letters, 29, 151

2000

10.1037/1082-989X.9.3.354

10.1007/s11136-007-9281-6

10.1080/00223890802107875

1997, Nonparametric models for polytomous responses

1971

2000

10.1080/13607860220142387

10.1006/drev.1994.1003

2009, American Journal of Lifestyle Medicine, 3, 6, 10.1177/1559827608326125

10.1006/drev.2001.0539

1989, Psychology and Aging, 4, 195, 10.1037/0882-7974.4.2.195

1999, Clinical Psychology, 6, 188

1995, Resilience and reserve capacity in later adulthood: potentials and limits of development across the life span, 2