Perceiving effort as poor learning: The misinterpreted-effort hypothesis of how experienced effort and perceived learning relate to study strategy choice
Tài liệu tham khảo
Agarwal, 2012, The value of applied research: Retrieval practice improves classroom learning and recommendations from a teacher, a principal, and a scientist, Educational Psychology Review, 24, 437, 10.1007/s10648-012-9210-2
Agarwal, 2017, Benefits from retrieval practice are greater for students with lower working memory capacity, Memory, 25, 764, 10.1080/09658211.2016.1220579
Alter, 2009, Uniting the tribes of fluency to form a metacognitive nation, Personality and Social Psychology Review, 13, 219, 10.1177/1088868309341564
American Museum of Natural History (2015). Unearthing pterosaurs. Retrieved from https://www.readworks.org/article/Unearthing-Pterosaurs/bb70e3b3-391d-4fa8-aec5-298afcddcb55.
Ariel, 2011, Habitual reading biases in the allocation of study time, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 1015, 10.3758/s13423-011-0128-3
Ariel, 2013, When do learners shift from habitual-based to agenda-based processes when selecting items for study?, Memory & Cognition, 41, 416, 10.3758/s13421-012-0267-4
Ariel, 2018, Improving self-regulated learning with a retrieval practice intervention, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 24, 43
Autin, 2012, Improving working memory efficiency by reframing metacognitive interpretation of task difficulty, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141, 610, 10.1037/a0027478
Baddeley, 1978, The influence of length and frequency of training session on the rate of learning to type, Ergonomics, 21, 627, 10.1080/00140137808931764
Barron, 2015, Expectancy-value-cost model of motivation, 503
Benjamin, 2006, Metacognitive control of the spacing of study repetitions, Journal of Memory and Language, 55, 126, 10.1016/j.jml.2006.02.003
Benjamin, 1998, Predicting the future and reconstructing the past: A Bayesian characterization of the utility of subjective fluency, Acta Psychologica, 98, 267, 10.1016/S0001-6918(97)00046-2
Benjamin, 1998, The mismeasure of memory: When retrieval fluency is misleading as a metamnemonic index, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127, 55, 10.1037/0096-3445.127.1.55
Besken, 2016, Picture-perfect is not perfect for metamamory: Testing the perceptual fluency hypothesis with degraded images, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 42, 1417
Besken, 2013, Easily perceived, easily remembered? Perceptual interference produces a double dissociation between metamemory and memory performance, Memory & Cognition, 41, 897, 10.3758/s13421-013-0307-8
Bielaczyc, 1995, Training in self-explanation and self-regulation strategies: Investigating the effects of knowledge acquisition activities on problem solving, Cognition and Instruction, 13, 221, 10.1207/s1532690xci1302_3
Birnbaum, 2013, Why interleaving enhances inductive learning: The roles of discrimination and retrieval, Memory & Cognition, 41, 392, 10.3758/s13421-012-0272-7
Bjork, 1994, Memory and metamemory considerations in the training of human beings, 185
Bjork, 1992, A new theory of disuse and an old theory of stimulus fluctuation, Vol. 2, 35
Bjork, 2013, Self-regulated learning: Beliefs, techniques, and illusions, Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 417, 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143823
Blaxton, 1989, Investigating dissociations among memory measures: Support for a transfer-appropriate processing framework, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 657
Broeren, 2018, Can instruction support the self-regulated use of retrieval practice in higher education?
Carpenter, 2009, Using tests to enhance 8th graders’ retention of U.S. history facts, Applied Cognitive Psychology, 23, 760, 10.1002/acp.1507
Carpenter, 2008, The effects of tests on learning and forgetting, Memory & Cognition, 36, 438, 10.3758/MC.36.2.438
Carpenter, 2013, Appearances can be deceiving: Instructor fluency increases perceptions of learning without increasing actual learning, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20, 1350, 10.3758/s13423-013-0442-z
Chevalier, 2018, Willing to think hard? The subjective value of cognitive effort in children, Child Development, 89, 1283, 10.1111/cdev.12805
Chi, 1989, Self-explanations: How students study and use examples in learning to solve problems, Cognitive Science, 13, 145, 10.1207/s15516709cog1302_1
Chi, 1994, Eliciting self-explanations improves understanding, Cognitive Science, 18, 439
Duckworth, 2011, Deliberate practice spells scucess: Why grittier competitors triumph at the National Spelling Bee, Social Psychological and Personality Science, 2, 174, 10.1177/1948550610385872
Duckworth, 2015, Measurement matters: Assessing personal qualities other than cognitive ability for educational purposes, Educational Researcher, 44, 237, 10.3102/0013189X15584327
Dunlosky, 1997, Older and younger adults use a functionally identical algorithm to select items for restudy during multitrial learning, Journal of Gerontology B: Psychological Sciences, 52, P178, 10.1093/geronb/52B.4.P178
Dunlosky, 2009
Dunlosky, 2013, Improving students’ learning with effective learning techniques: Promising directions from cognitive and educational psychology, Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 14, 4, 10.1177/1529100612453266
Dweck, 1999
Eccles, 2002, Motivational beliefs, values, and goals, Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 109, 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135153
Einstein, 2012, The testing effect: Illustrating a fundamental concept and changing study strategies, Teaching of Psychology, 39, 190, 10.1177/0098628312450432
Eskreis-Winkler, 2016, Using wise interventions to motivate deliberate practice, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 111, 728, 10.1037/pspp0000074
Faul, 2007, G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences, Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175, 10.3758/BF03193146
Fiechter, 2018, Audiovisual quality impacts assessments of job candidates in video interviews: Evidence for an AV quality bias, Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications, 3, 47
Finley, 2012, Adaptive and qualitative changes in encoding strategy with experience: Evidence from the test-expectancy paradigm, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38, 632
Finley, 2010, Metacognitive Control of Learning and Remembering, 109
Finn, 2015, When confidence is not a signal of knowing: How students’ experiences and beliefs about processing fluency can lead to miscalibrated confidence, Educational Psychology Review, 27, 567, 10.1007/s10648-015-9313-7
Flake, 2015, Measuring cost: The forgotten component of expectancy-value theory, Contemporary Educational Psychology, 41, 232, 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2015.03.002
Fraundorf, 2014, Knowing the crowd within: Metacognitive limits on combining multiple judgments, Journal of Memory and Language, 71, 17, 10.1016/j.jml.2013.10.002
Fraundorf, 2015, Conflict and metacognitive control: The mismatch-monitoring hypothesis of how others’ knowledge states affect recall, Memory, 24, 1108, 10.1080/09658211.2015.1069853
Galla, 2014, The Academic Diligence Task (ADT): Assessing individual differences in effort on tedious but important schoolwork, Contemporary Educational Psychology, 39, 314, 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2014.08.001
Geller, 2018, Study strategies and beliefs about learning as a function of academic achievement and achievement goals, Memory, 26, 683, 10.1080/09658211.2017.1397175
Guo, 2016, Probing the unique contributions of self-concept, task values, and their interactions using multiple value facets and multiple academic outcomes, AERA Open, 2, 1, 10.1177/2332858415626884
Hacker, 2000, Test prediction and performance in a classroom context, Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 160, 10.1037/0022-0663.92.1.160
Hall, 1994, Contextual interference effects with skilled baseball players, Perceptual and Motor Skills, 78, 835, 10.1177/003151259407800331
Hartwig, 2012, Study strategies of college students: Are self-testing and scheduling related to achievement?, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 126, 10.3758/s13423-011-0181-y
Hawkins, 2012, An optimal adjustment procedure to minimize experiment time in decisions with multiple alternatives, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 339, 10.3758/s13423-012-0216-z
Hawkins, 2011, Leave the experiment as quickly as possible, without looking stupid: An optimal adjustment procedure to explain context effects in multi-alternative choice
Hayes, 2013
Heine, 2001, Divergent consequences of success and failure in Japan and North America: An investigation of self-improving motivations and malleable selves, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 599, 10.1037/0022-3514.81.4.599
Hinze, 2011, Testing the limits of testing effects using completion tests, Memory, 19, 290, 10.1080/09658211.2011.560121
Hofmann, 2008, Impulsive versus reflective influences on health behavior: A theoretical framework and empirical review, Health Psychology Review, 2, 111, 10.1080/17437190802617668
Inzlicht, 2018, The effort paradox: Effort is both costly and valued, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22, 337, 10.1016/j.tics.2018.01.007
Jacoby, 1989, An illusion of memory: False recognition influenced by unconscious perception, Journal of Experimetnal Psychology: General, 118, 126, 10.1037/0096-3445.118.2.126
Job, 2010, Ego depletion-is it all in your head? Implicit theories about willpower affect self-regulation, Psychological Science, 21, 1686, 10.1177/0956797610384745
Job, 2015, Effects of practicing self-control on academic performance, Motivation Science, 1, 219, 10.1037/mot0000024
Kang, 2012, Learning painting styles: Spacing is advantageous when it promotes discriminative contrast, Applied Cognitive Psychology, 26, 97, 10.1002/acp.1801
Karpicke, 2009, Metacognitive control and strategy selection: Deciding to practice retrieval during learning, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138, 469, 10.1037/a0017341
Karpicke, 2012, Retrieval-based learning: Active retrieval promotes meaningful learning, Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 157, 10.1177/0963721412443552
Karpicke, 2015, The testing effect is alive and well with complex materials, Educational Psychology Review, 27, 317, 10.1007/s10648-015-9309-3
Karpicke, 2007, Repeated retrieval during learning is the key to long-term retention, Journal of Memory and Language, 57, 151, 10.1016/j.jml.2006.09.004
Karpicke, 2008, The critical importance of retrieval for learning, Science, 319, 966, 10.1126/science.1152408
Kelley, 1996, Adult egocentrism: Subjective experience versus analytic bases for judgment, Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 157, 10.1006/jmla.1996.0009
Koriat, 1997, Monitoring one’s own knowledge during study: A cue-utilization approach to judgments of learning, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 126, 349, 10.1037/0096-3445.126.4.349
Koriat, 2008, Easy comes, easy goes?: The link between learning and remembering and its exploitation in metacognition, Memory & Cognition, 36, 416, 10.3758/MC.36.2.416
Koriat, 2014, The effects of goal-driven and data-driven regulation of metacognitive monitoring during learning: A developmental perspective, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 386, 10.1037/a0031768
Koriat, 2005, Illusions of competence in monitoring one’s knowledge during study, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 187
Koriat, 2004, Predicting one’s own forgetting: The role of experience-based and theory-based processes, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 643, 10.1037/0096-3445.133.4.643
Koriat, 2014, Judgments of learning depend on how learners interpret study effort, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40, 1624
Koriat, 2002, Comparing objective and subjective learning curves: Judgments of learning exhibit increased underconfidence with practice, Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 131, 147, 10.1037/0096-3445.131.2.147
Kornell, 2007, The promise and perils of self-regulated study, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 219, 10.3758/BF03194055
Kornell, 2008, Learning concepts and categories: Is spacing the enemy of induction?, Psychological Science, 19, 585, 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02127.x
Kornell, 2008, Optimising self-regulated study: The benefits—and costs—of dropping flashcards, Memory, 16, 125, 10.1080/09658210701763899
Kornell, 2009, A stability bias in human memory: Overestimating remembering and underestimating learning, Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 138, 449, 10.1037/a0017350
Kornell, 2010, Spacing as the friend of both memory and induction in young and older adults, Psychology and Aging, 25, 498, 10.1037/a0017807
Kornell, 2011, The ease-of-processing heuristic and the stability bias: Dissociating memory, memory beliefs, and memory judgments, Psychological Science, 22, 787, 10.1177/0956797611407929
Kornell, 2009, Learners’ choices and beliefs about self-testing, Memory, 17, 493, 10.1080/09658210902832915
Kurtz, 1956, Concept learning with differing sequences of instances, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51, 239, 10.1037/h0040295
Kurzban, 2013, An opportunity cost model of subjective effort and task performance, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36, 661, 10.1017/S0140525X12003196
Loewenstein, 1996, Out of control: Visceral influences on behavior, Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 65, 272, 10.1006/obhd.1996.0028
Logan, 2012, Metacognition and the spacing effect: The role of repetition, feedback, and instruction on judgments of learning for massed and spaced rehearsal, Metacognition and Learning, 7, 175, 10.1007/s11409-012-9090-3
Mann, 2013, Self-regulation of health behavior: Social psychological approaches to goal setting and goal striving, Health Psychology, 32, 487, 10.1037/a0028533
McCabe, 2011, Metacognitive awareness of learning strategies in undergraduates, Memory & Cognition, 39, 462, 10.3758/s13421-010-0035-2
McDaniel, 2007, Testing the testing effect in the classroom, European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 19, 494, 10.1080/09541440701326154
McDermott, 2014, Both multiple-choice and short-answer quizzes enhance later exam performance in middle and high school classes, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 20, 3
McNamara, 2004, SERT: Self-explanation reading training, Discourse Processes, 38, 1, 10.1207/s15326950dp3801_1
McNamara, 2004, iSTART: Interactive strategy training for active reading and thinking, Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36, 222, 10.3758/BF03195567
McNamara, 2006, Improving adolescent students’ reading comprehension with iSTART, Journal of Educational Computing Research, 34, 147, 10.2190/1RU5-HDTJ-A5C8-JVWE
Metcalfe, 2008, Evidence that judgments of learning are causally related to study choice, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 174, 10.3758/PBR.15.1.174
Miele, 2011, Does easily learned mean easily Remembered? It depends on your beliefs about intelligence, Psychological Science, 22, 320, 10.1177/0956797610397954
Mrazek, 2018, Expanding minds: Growth mindsets of self-regulation and the influences on effort and perseverance, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 79, 164, 10.1016/j.jesp.2018.07.003
Mueller, 2014, The font-size effect on judgments of learning: Does it exemplify fluency effects or reflect people’s beliefs about memory?, Journal of Memory and Language, 70, 1, 10.1016/j.jml.2013.09.007
Nelson, 1991, When people’s judgments of learning (JOLs) are extremely accurate at predicting subsequent recall: The “delayed-JOL effect”, Psychological Science, 2, 267, 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1991.tb00147.x
Nelson, 1988, Allocation of self-paced study time and the “labor-in-vain effect”, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14, 676
Nelson, 1990, Metamemory: A theoretical framework and new findings, 125, 10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60053-5
Nungester, 1982, Testing versus review: Effects on retention, Journal of Education Psychology, 74, 18, 10.1037/0022-0663.74.1.18
Pashler, 2007, Enhancing learning and retarding forgetting: Choices and consequences, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 187, 10.3758/BF03194050
Peer, 2011, Too reliable to be true? Response bias as a potential source of inflation in paper-and-pencil questionnaire reliability, Practical Assessment, Research & Evaluation, 16
Perez, 2014, The role of identity development, values, and costs in college STEM retention, Journal of Educational Psychology, 106, 315, 10.1037/a0034027
Plaut, 2005, The “inside” story: A cultural-historical analysis of being smart and motivated, American style, 457
Poropat, 2009, A meta-analysis of the five-factor model of personality and academic performance, Psychological Bulletin, 135, 322, 10.1037/a0014996
Postman, 1965, Retention as a function of degree of overlearning, Science, 135, 666, 10.1126/science.135.3504.666
Pyc, 2010, Toward an understanding of students’ allocation of study time: Why do they decide to mass or space their practice?, Memory & Cognition, 38, 431, 10.3758/MC.38.4.431
Pyc, 2011, Costs and benefits of dropout schedules of test-restudy practice: Implications for student learning, Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25, 87, 10.1002/acp.1646
Rawson, 2002, Are performance predictions for text based on ease of processing?, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28, 69
Rawson, 2011, Optimizing schedules of retrieval practice for durable and efficient learning; How much is enough?, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140, 283, 10.1037/a0023956
ReadWorks, Inc. (2013a). The American chestnut. Retrieved from https://www.readworks.org/article/The-American-Chestnut/94810bf5-e192-4c2a-a1fe-35ba9af41cd3.
ReadWorks, Inc. (2013b). The Great Barrier Reef. Retrieved from https://www.readworks.org/article/The-Great-Barrier-Reef/0e48bb0e-8759-403f-ad6c-0493101ea727.
ReadWorks, Inc. (2013c). Water on the Earth. Retrieved from https://www.readworks.org/article/Water-on-the-Earth/c9935af1-b930-438e-b5ca-c1a4beb3f554.
Rhodes, 2008, Memory predictions are influenced by perceptual information: Evidence for metacognitive illusions, Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 137, 615, 10.1037/a0013684
Rhodes, 2009, Metacognitive illusions for auditory information: Effects on monitoring and control, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 550, 10.3758/PBR.16.3.550
Roediger, 2006, Test-enhanced learning: Taking memory tests imporves long-term retention, Psychological Science, 17, 249, 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01693.x
Roediger, 2012, Inexpensive techniques to improve education: Applying cognitive psychology to enhance educational practice, Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 1, 242, 10.1016/j.jarmac.2012.09.002
Rohrer, 2014, The benefit of interleaved mathematics practice is not limited to superficially similar kinds of problems, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21, 1323, 10.3758/s13423-014-0588-3
Rohrer, 2010, Recent research on human learning challenges conventional instructional strategies, Educational Researcher, 39, 406, 10.3102/0013189X10374770
Rohrer, 2007, The shuffling of mathematics problems improves learning, Instructional Science, 35, 481, 10.1007/s11251-007-9015-8
Rothman, 2015, Hale and hearty policies: How psychological science can create and maintain healthy habits, Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10, 701, 10.1177/1745691615598515
Rowland, 2014, The effect of testing versus restudy on retention: A meta-analytic review of the testing effect, Psychological Bulletin, 140, 1, 10.1037/a0037559
Schmidt, 1992, New conceptualizations of practice: Common principles in three paradigms suggest new concepts for training, Psychological Science, 3, 207, 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1992.tb00029.x
Scholer, 2018, New directions in self-regulation: The role of metamotivational beliefs, Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27, 437, 10.1177/0963721418790549
Schunn, 2009, Real-time NIRS feedback may help improve self-explanation learning strategy use
Schwartz, 1997, The inferential and experiental basis of metamemory, Current Directions in Psychological Science, 6, 132, 10.1111/1467-8721.ep10772899
Serra, 2010, Metacomprehension judgements reflect the belief that diagrams improve learning from text, Memory, 18, 698, 10.1080/09658211.2010.506441
Shenhav, 2017, Toward a rational and mechanistic account of mental effort, Annual Review of Neuroscience, 40, 99, 10.1146/annurev-neuro-072116-031526
Simons, 2011, What people believe about how memory works: A representative sample of the U.S. population, PLoS ONE, 6, e22757, 10.1371/journal.pone.0022757
Simons, 2012, Common (mis)beliefs about memory: A replication and comparison of telephone and Mechanical Turk survey methods, PLoS ONE, 7, e51876, 10.1371/journal.pone.0051876
Smith, 2015, Just not worth my time? Experienced difficulty and time investment, Social Cognition, 33, 1, 10.1521/soco.2015.33.2.1
Soderstrom, 2015, Learning versus performance: An integrative view, Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10, 176, 10.1177/1745691615569000
Son, 2004, Spacing one’s study: Evidence for a metacognitive control strategy, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 601
Son, 2010, Metacognitive control and the spacing effect, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 255
Son, 2000, Metacognitive and control strategies in study-time allocation, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 204
Sungkhasettee, 2011, Memory and metamemory for inverted words: Illusions of competency and desirable difficulties, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 973, 10.3758/s13423-011-0114-9
Susser, 2016, Identity priming consistently affects perceptual fluency but only affects metamemory when primes are obvious, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 42, 657
Susser, 2013, From the lab to the dorm room: Metacognitive awareness and use of spaced study, Instructional Science, 41, 345, 10.1007/s11251-012-9231-8
Taylor, 2010, The effects of interleaved practice, Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24, 837, 10.1002/acp.1598
Toppino, 2009, Metacognitive control over the distribution of practice: When is spacing preferred?, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 1352
Tullis, 2011, On the effectiveness of self-paced learning, Journal of Memory and Language, 64, 109, 10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.002
Tullis, 2011, A metacognitive illusion in category learning
Tullis, 2013, Metacognition of the testing effect: Guiding learners to predict the benefits of retrieval, Memory & Cognition, 41, 429, 10.3758/s13421-012-0274-5
Undorf, 2011, Judgments of learning reflect encoding fluency: Conclusive effort in favor of the ease-of-processing hypothesis, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37, 1264
University of Pittsburgh (2016). Pitt football through the years. Retrieved from http://exhibit.library.pitt.edu/football-through-years/.
University of Pittsburgh (2017). Preserving our architectural treasures. Retrieved from http://www.treasures.pitt.edu/history/.
Unkelbach, 2006, The learned interpretation of cognitive fluency, Psychological Science, 17, 339, 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01708.x
van Gog, 2015, Not new, but nearly forgotten: The testing effect decreases or even disappears as the complexity of learning materials increases, Educational Psychology Review, 27, 247, 10.1007/s10648-015-9310-x
Vaughn, 2011, Diagnosing criterion-level effects on memory: What aspects of memory are enhanced by repeated retrieval?, Psychological Science, 22, 1127, 10.1177/0956797611417724
Wahlheim, 2011, Spacing enhances the learning of natural concepts: An investigation of mechanisms, metacognition, and aging, Memory & Cognition, 39, 750, 10.3758/s13421-010-0063-y
Wahlheim, 2012, Metacognitive judgments of repetition and variability effects in natural concept learning: Evidence for variability neglect, Memory & Cognition, 40, 703, 10.3758/s13421-011-0180-2
Weekly Reader Corporation (2009). Short circuits. Retrieved from https://www.readworks.org/article/Short-Circuits/ff97e001-bccc-41d5-b53a-58015dfe8995#!.
Westbrook, 2015, Cognitive effort: A neuroeconomic approach, Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 15, 395, 10.3758/s13415-015-0334-y
Westbrook, 2013, What is the subjective cost of cognitive effort? Load, trait, and aging effects revealed by economic preference, PLoS ONE, 8, e68210, 10.1371/journal.pone.0068210
Winne, 1998, Studying as self-regulated learning, 277
Yan, 2014
Yan, 2016, On the difficulty of mending metacognitive illusions: A priori theories, fluency effects, and misattributions of the interleaving benefit, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145, 918, 10.1037/xge0000177
Yan, 2017, How should exemplars be sequenced in inductive learning? Empirical evidence versus learners’ opinions, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 23, 403
Yan, 2014, Habits and beliefs that guide self-regulated learning: Do they vary with mindset?, Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 3, 140, 10.1037/h0101799
Yeager, 2012, Mindsets that promote resilience: When students believe that personal characteristics can be developed, Educational Psychologist, 47, 302, 10.1080/00461520.2012.722805
Yeager, 2014, Boring but important: A self-transcendent purpose for learning fosters academic self-regulation, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107, 559, 10.1037/a0037637
Yeager, 2011, Social-psychological interventions in education: They’re not magic, Review of Educational Research, 81, 267, 10.3102/0034654311405999
Yue, 2013, When disfluency is—and is not—a desirable difficulty: The influence of typeface clarity on metacognitive judgments and memory, Memory & Cognition, 41, 229, 10.3758/s13421-012-0255-8
Zimmerman, 2000, Self-efficacy: An essential motive to learn, Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25, 82, 10.1006/ceps.1999.1016
Zulkiply, 2012, Spacing and induction: Application to exemplars presented as auditory and visual text, Learning and Instruction, 22, 215, 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2011.11.002