ORCA: A picture database of object–scene arrangements for cross-cultural and aging research
Tóm tắt
In recent years, cross-cultural research on the modulation of basic cognitive processes by culture has intensified – also from an aging perspective. Despite this increased research interest, only a few cross-culturally normed non-verbal stimulus sets are available to support cross-cultural cognitive research in younger and older adults. Here we present the ORCA (Official Rating of Complex Arrangements) picture database, which includes a total of 720 object–scene compositions sorted into 180 quadruples (e.g., two different helmets placed in two different deserts). Each quadruple contains visually and semantically matched pairs of objects and pairs of scenes with varying degrees of semantic fit between objects and scenes. A total of 95 younger and older German and Chinese adults rated every object–scene pair on object familiarity and semantic fit between object and scene. While the ratings were significantly correlated between cultures and age groups, small but significant culture and age differences emerged. Object familiarity was higher for older adults than younger adults and for German participants than for Chinese participants. Semantic fit was rated lower by German older adults and Chinese younger adults as compared to German younger adults and Chinese older adults. Due to the large number of stimuli, our database is particularly well suited for cognitive and neuroscientific research on cross-cultural and age-related differences in perception, attention, and memory.
Từ khóa
Tài liệu tham khảo
Bates, E., D’Amico, S., Jacobsen, T., Székely, A., Andonova, E., Devescovi, A., Herron, D., Ching Lu, C., Pechmann, T., Pléh, C., Wicha, N., Federmeier, K., Gerdjikova, I., Gutierrez, G., Hung, D., Hsu, J., Iyer, G., Kohnert, K., Mehotcheva, T. … Tzeng, O. (2003). Timed picture naming in seven languages. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10(2), 344–380. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196494
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 48. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01
Ben-Shachar, M. S., Lüdecke, D., & Makowski, D. (2020). effectsize: Estimation of Effect Size Indices and Standardized Parameters. Journal of Open Source Software, 5(56), 2815. https://doi.org/10.21105/joss.02815
Borges, M. T., Fernandes, E. G., & Coco, M. I. (2020). Age-related differences during visual search: The role of contextual expectations and cognitive control mechanisms. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 27(4), 489–516. https://doi.org/10.1080/13825585.2019.1632256
Brady, T. F., Konkle, T., Alvarez, G. A., & Oliva, A. (2008). Visual long-term memory has a massive storage capacity for object details. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(38), 14325–14329. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0803390105
Bridger, E. K., Kursawe, A.-L., Bader, R., Tibon, R., Gronau, N., Levy, D. A., & Mecklinger, A. (2017). Age effects on associative memory for novel picture pairings. Brain Research, 1664, 102–115. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2017.03.031
Brodeur, M. B., O’Sullivan, M., & Crone, L. (2017). The impact of image format and normative variables on episodic memory. Cogent Psychology, 4(1), 1328869. https://doi.org/10.1080/23311908.2017.1328869
Choi, I., Koo, M., & Choi, J. A. (2007). Individual differences in analytic versus holistic thinking. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33(5), 691–705. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167206298568
Chua, H. F., Boland, J. E., & Nisbett, R. E. (2005). Cultural variation in eye movements during scene perception. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102(35), 12629–12633. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0506162102
Davenport, J. L. (2007). Consistency effects between objects in scenes. Memory & Cognition, 35(3), 393–401. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193280
Denkhaus, R., & Bös, M. (2012). How cultural is ‘cultural neuroscience’? Some comments on an emerging research paradigm. BioSocieties, 7(4), 433–458. https://doi.org/10.1057/biosoc.2012.30
Duñabeitia, J. A., Crepaldi, D., Meyer, A. S., New, B., Pliatsikas, C., Smolka, E., & Brysbaert, M. (2018). MultiPic: A standardized set of 750 drawings with norms for six European languages. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71(4), 808–816. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2017.1310261
Evans, K., Rotello, C. M., Li, X., & Rayner, K. (2009). Scene perception and memory revealed by eye movements and receiver-operating characteristic analyses: Does a cultural difference truly exist? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(2), 276–285. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210802373720
Field, A. P., Miles, J., & Field, Z. (2012). Discovering statistics using R. Sage.
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12(3), 189–198. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6
Fox, J., & Weisberg, S. (2019). An R Companion to Applied Regression (3rd ed.). Sage.
Goh, J. (2010). Object-Scenes. Stimuli collection, PAL Stimuli Database. http://agingmind.utdallas.edu/stimuli/object-scenedb
Goto, S. G., Ando, Y., Huang, C., Yee, A., & Lewis, R. S. (2010). Cultural differences in the visual processing of meaning: Detecting incongruities between background and foreground objects using the N400. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 5(2–3), 242–253. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsp038
Goto, S. G., Yee, A., Lowenberg, K., & Lewis, R. S. (2013). Cultural differences in sensitivity to social context: Detecting affective incongruity using the N400. Social Neuroscience, 8(1), 63–74. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2012.739202
Guez, J., & Lev, D. (2016). A picture is worth a thousand words? Not when it comes to associative memory of older adults. Psychology and Aging, 31(1), 37–41. https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000069
Gutchess, A., & Huff, S. (2016). Cross-cultural differences in memory. In J. Y. Chiao, S.-C. Li, R. Seligman, & R. Turner (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of cultural neuroscience (pp. 155–169). Oxford University Press.
Han, S., & Ma, Y. (2014). Cultural differences in human brain activity: A quantitative meta-analysis. NeuroImage, 99, 293–300. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.05.062
Harrell Jr., F. (2022). Hmisc: Harrell Miscellaneous. R package version 4.7-0. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=Hmisc
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). Most people are not WEIRD. Nature, 466(7302), 29–29. https://doi.org/10.1038/466029a
Hofstede, G. (2001). Culture’s consequences: Comparing values, behaviors, institutions, and organizations across nations (2nd ed.). Sage.
Huffer, V., Bader, R., & Mecklinger, A. (2022). Can the elderly take the action? – The influence of unitization induced by action relationships on the associative memory deficit. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 194, 107655. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2022.107655
Jung, T.-P., Makeig, S., Westerfield, M., Townsend, J., Courchesne, E., & Sejnowski, T. J. (2000). Removal of eye activity artifacts from visual event-related potentials in normal and clinical subjects. Clinical Neurophysiology, 111(10), 1745–1758. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1388-2457(00)00386-2
Kay, M., Elkin, L., Higgins, J., & Wobbrock, J. (2021). ARTool: Aligned Rank Transform for Nonparametric Factorial ANOVAs. Zenondo. https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.594511
Kim, H., & Markus, H. R. (1999). Deviance or uniqueness, harmony or conformity? A cultural analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(4), 785–800. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.77.4.785
Kitayama, S., Duffy, S., Kawamura, T., & Larsen, J. T. (2003). Perceiving an object and its context in different cultures: A cultural look at new look. Psychological Science, 14(3), 201–206. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.02432
Kitayama, S., King, A., Yoon, C., Tompson, S., Huff, S., & Liberzon, I. (2014). The Dopamine D4 Receptor Gene (DRD4) moderates cultural difference in independent versus interdependent social orientation. Psychological Science, 25(6), 1169–1177. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614528338
Ko, S.-G., Lee, T.-H., Yoon, H.-Y., Kwon, J.-H., & Mather, M. (2011). How does context affect assessments of facial emotion? The role of culture and age. Psychology and Aging, 26(1), 48–59. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020222
Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. (2017). lmerTest Package: Tests in linear mixed effects models. Journal of Statistical Software, 82, 1–26. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v082.i13
Lang, P. J., & Bradley, M. M. (2005). International affective picture system (IAPS): Digitized photographs, instruction manual and affective ratings (Technical Report A-6). University of Florida, Gainesville, FL.
Lawrence, M. A. (2016). ez: Easy Analysis and Visualization of Factorial Experiments. R package version 4.4-0. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=ez
Loftus, G. R., & Bell, S. M. (1975). Two types of information in picture memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 1(2), 103–113. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.1.2.103
Luck, S. J. (2014). An introduction to the event-related potential technique (2nd ed.). The MIT Press.
Masuda, T. (2017). Culture and attention: Recent empirical findings and new directions in cultural psychology. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 11(12), e12363. https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12363
Masuda, T., & Nisbett, R. E. (2001). Attending holistically versus analytically: Comparing the context sensitivity of Japanese and Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(5), 922–934. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.81.5.922
Masuda, T., Ellsworth, P. C., Mesquita, B., Leu, J., Tanida, S., & Van de Veerdonk, E. (2008). Placing the face in context: Cultural differences in the perception of facial emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(3), 365–381. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.94.3.365
Mecklinger, A., & Bader, R. (2020). From fluency to recognition decisions: A broader view of familiarity-based remembering. Neuropsychologia, 146, 107527. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107527
Mecklinger, A., Kriukova, O., Mühlmann, H., & Grunwald, T. (2014). Cross-cultural differences in processing of architectural ranking: Evidence from an event-related potential study. Cognitive Neuroscience, 5(1), 45–53. https://doi.org/10.1080/17588928.2013.869740
Mickley Steinmetz, K. R., Sturkie, C. M., Rochester, N. M., Liu, X., & Gutchess, A. H. (2018). Cross-cultural differences in item and background memory: Examining the influence of emotional intensity and scene congruency. Memory, 26(6), 751–758. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2017.1406119
Mohr, J., Seyfarth, J., Lueschow, A., Weber, J. E., Wichmann, F. A., & Obermayer, K. (2016). BOiS—Berlin Object in Scene Database: Controlled Photographic Images for Visual Search Experiments with Quantified Contextual Priors. Frontiers in Psychology, 7. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00749
Navarro, D. J. (2015). Learning statistics with R: A tutorial for psychology students and other beginners. (Version 0.6). University of New South Wales. Sydney, Australia
Naveh-Benjamin, M. (2000). Adult age differences in memory performance: Tests of an associative deficit hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26(5), 1170–1187. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.26.5.1170
Naveh-Benjamin, M., & Mayr, U. (2018). Age-related differences in associative memory: Empirical evidence and theoretical perspectives. Psychology and Aging, 33(1), 1–6. https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000235
Naveh-Benjamin, M., Hussain, Z., Guez, J., & Bar-On, M. (2003). Adult age differences in episodic memory: Further support for an associative-deficit hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29(5), 826–837. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.29.5.826
Nisbett, R. E. (2003). The geography of thought: How Asians and Westerners think differently ... and why. Free Press.
Nisbett, R. E., & Masuda, T. (2003). Culture and point of view. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100(19), 11163–11170. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1934527100
Öhlschläger, S., & Võ, M. L.-H. (2017). SCEGRAM: An image database for semantic and syntactic inconsistencies in scenes. Behavior Research Methods, 49(5), 1780–1791. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-016-0820-3
Old, S. R., & Naveh-Benjamin, M. (2008). Differential effects of age on item and associative measures of memory: A meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging, 23(1), 104–118. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.23.1.104
Palmer, S. E. (1975). The effects of contextual scenes on the identification of objects. Memory & Cognition, 3(5), 519–526. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03197524
Park, D., & Gutchess, A. (2006). The Cognitive Neuroscience of Aging and Culture. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15(3), 105–108. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2006.00416.x
Park, D. C., Nisbett, R., & Hedden, T. (1999). Aging, Culture, and Cognition. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, 54B(2), P75–P84. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/54B.2.P75
R Core Team. (2022). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing https://www.R-project.org/
Rossion, B., & Pourtois, G. (2004). Revisiting Snodgrass and Vanderwart’s object pictorial set: The role of surface detail in basic-level object recognition. Perception, 33(2), 217–236. https://doi.org/10.1068/p5117
Scariano, S. M., & Davenport, J. M. (1987). The effects of violations of independence assumptions in the one-way ANOVA. The American Statistician, 41(2), 123–129. https://doi.org/10.2307/2684223
Singelis, T. M. (1994). The measurement of independent and interdependent self-construals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20(5), 580–591. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167294205014
Singelis, T. M., & Sharkey, W. F. (1995). Culture, self-construal, and embarrassability. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 26(6), 622–644. https://doi.org/10.1177/002202219502600607
Snodgrass, J. G., & Vanderwart, M. (1980). A standardized set of 260 pictures: Norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6(2), 174–215. https://doi.org/10.1037//0278-7393.6.2.174
Souza, C., Garrido, M. V., & Carmo, J. C. (2020). A Systematic Review of Normative Studies Using Images of Common Objects. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 573314. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.573314
Stark, S. M., Kirwan, C. B., & Stark, C. E. L. (2019). Mnemonic Similarity Task: A Tool for Assessing Hippocampal Integrity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23(11), 938–951. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2019.08.003
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics (5th ed.). Pearson/Allyn & Bacon.
Varnum, M. E. W., Grossmann, I., Kitayama, S., & Nisbett, R. E. (2010). The origin of cultural differences in cognition: The social orientation hypothesis. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19(1), 9–13. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721409359301
Weigl, M., Pham, H. H., Mecklinger, A., & Rosburg, T. (2020). The effect of shared distinctiveness on source memory: An event-related potential study. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 20(5), 1027–1040. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-020-00817-1
Wickham, H., Averick, M., Bryan, J., Chang, W., McGowan, L. D., François, R., Grolemund, G., Hayes, A., Henry, L., Hester, J., Kuhn, M., Pedersen, T. L., Miller, E., Bache, S. M., Müller, K., Ooms, J., Robinson, D., Seidel, D. P., Spinu, V., … Yutani, H. (2019). Welcome to the Tidyverse. Journal of Open Source Software, 4(43), 1686. https://doi.org/10.21105/joss.01686
Wilcox, C. E., & Claus, E. D. (2017). The importance of standardization of stimuli for functional MRI tasks to evaluate substance use disorder pathology. The American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 43(6), 625–627. https://doi.org/10.1080/00952990.2017.1299745
Wobbrock, J. O., Findlater, L., Gergle, D., & Higgins, J. J. (2011). The aligned rank transform for nonparametric factorial analyses using only ANOVA procedures. Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, 143–146. https://doi.org/10.1145/1978942.1978963
Yoon, C., Hasher, L., Feinberg, F., Rahhal, T. A., & Winocur, G. (2000). Cross-cultural differences in memory: The role of culture-based stereotypes about aging. Psychology and Aging, 15(4), 694–704. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.15.4.694
Yoon, C., Feinberg, F., Luo, T., Hedden, T., Gutchess, A. H., Chen, H.-Y. M., Mikels, J. A., Jiao, S., & Park, D. C. (2004). A cross-culturally standardized set of pictures for younger and older adults: American and Chinese norms for name agreement, concept agreement, and familiarity. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36(4), 639–649. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03206545
Zhang, Z. & Mai Y. (2022). WebPower: Basic and Advanced Statistical Power Analysis. R package version 0.7. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=WebPower
Zhang, X., Fung, H. H., Stanley, J. T., Isaacowitz, D. M., & Zhang, Q. (2014). Thinking more holistically as we grow older? Results from different tasks in two cultures. Culture and Brain, 2(2), 109–121. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40167-014-0018-4