Neuroanatomical substrates of maximizing tendency in decision-making: a voxel-based morphometric study
Tóm tắt
Maximizing tendency is a central decision-making concept that has increasingly drawn attention from the scientific community. It refers to individuals’ predisposition to look for the best option instead of settling for something that merely passes an internal threshold of acceptability. Although this maximizing strategy intuitively increases individual benefits, it also has been linked to various negative outcomes, including decreased well-being and low life satisfaction, and it varies considerably across populations. However, the neuroanatomical characteristics underlying this heterogeneity remain poorly understood. To address this knowledge gap, a 13-item Maximization Scale and magnetic resonance imaging technique were respectively used in this study to estimate individual maximizing tendency and structural morphological information on a sample of healthy adults (n = 69). Furthermore, voxel-based morphometry (VBM) analysis was conducted to investigate the associations between gray matter volume (GMV) and maximizing tendency through univariate and multivariate pattern analysis (MVPA). Univariate analysis did not determine an association between maximizing tendency and whole-brain GMV; by contrast, MVPA revealed that maximizing tendency could be successfully predicted by the GMVs of the right inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), right insula, and right cerebellum. These findings suggest the critical role of the morphological characteristics of the cortical-subcortical circuitry in individuals’ maximizing tendency.
Tài liệu tham khảo
Aron, A. R., Fletcher, P. C., Bullmore, E. T., Sahakian, B. J., & Robbins, T. W. (2003). Stop-signal inhibition disrupted by damage to right inferior frontal gyrus in humans. Nature Neuroscience, 6(2), 115–116. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1003
Aron, A. R., Robbins, T. W., & Poldrack, R. A. (2004). Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(4), 170–177. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.02.010
Awo, L. O., Oko, C. A., Yahaya, A., & Chukwu, P. C. (2019). Counterfactual Thinking and Gender Difference Effect on Voting Decision. III(X), 51–56
Bruin, W. B., De, Dombrovski, A. Y., Parker, A. M., & Szanto, K. (2015). Late-life Depression, Suicidal Ideation, and Attempted Suicide : The Role of Individual Differences in Maximizing, Regret, and Negative Decision Outcomes. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.1882
de Bruine, W., Parker, A. M., & Strough, J. (2016). Choosing to Be Happy? Age Differences in Maximizingg Decision Strategies and Experienced Emotional Well-Being. SSRN Electronic Journal. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.2744764
Carlisi, C. O., Norman, L. J., Lukito, S. S., Radua, J., Mataix-Cols, D., & Rubia, K. (2017). Comparative Multimodal Meta-analysis of Structural and Functional Brain Abnormalities in Autism Spectrum Disorder and Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 82(2), 83–102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.10.006
Chua, H. F., Gonzalez, R., Taylor, S. F., Welsh, R. C., & Liberzon, I. (2009). Decision-related loss: Regret and disappointment. NeuroImage, 47(4), 2031–2040. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.006
College, S., Schwartz, B., College, S., Morrin, M., & Schwartz, B. (2008). A Short Form Of The Maximization Scale: Factor Structure, Reliability And Validity Studies. 3(5)
Cui, Z., Su, M., Li, L., Shu, H., & Gong, G. (2018). Individualized Prediction of Reading Comprehension Ability Using Gray Matter Volume. Cerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y.: 1991), 28(5), 1656–1672. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhx061
Dar-nimrod, I., Rawn, C. D., Lehman, D. R., & Schwartz, B. (2009). The Maximization Paradox: The costs of seeking alternatives q. Personality and Individual Differences, 46(5–6), 631–635. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2009.01.007
De Bruin, W., Bruine, Parker, A. M., & Fischhoff, B. (2007). Individual differences in adult decision-making competence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(5), 938–956. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.92.5.938
Diab, D. L., Gillespie, M. A., & Highhouse, S. (2008). Are maximizers really unhappy ? The measurement of maximizing tendency. 3(5), 364–370
Dixon, M. L., Thiruchselvam, R., Todd, R., & Christoff, K. (2017). Emotion and the Prefrontal Cortex: An Integrative Review Challenges in Understanding the Role of the PFC in Emotion. Psychological Bulletin, 143(10), 1033–1081
Drucker, H., Surges, C. J. C., Kaufman, L., Smola, A., & Vapnik, V. (1997). Support vector regression machines. Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems, May 2018, 155–161
Fellows, L. K. (2004). The cognitive neuroscience of human decision making: a review and conceptual framework. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 3(3), 159–172. https://doi.org/10.1177/1534582304273251
Furnham, A., & Dowsett, T. (1993). Sex difference in social comparison and uniqueness bias. Personality and Individual Differences, 15(2), 175–183. https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(93)90024-W
Fushiki, T. (2011). Estimation of prediction error by using K-fold cross-validation. Statistics and Computing, 21(2), 137–146. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11222-009-9153-8
Gogolla, N. (2017). The insular cortex. Current Biology, 27(12), R580–R586. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2017.05.010
Guimond, S., Chatard, A., Martinot, D., Crisp, R. J., & Redersdorff, S. (2006). Social comparison, self-stereotyping, and gender differences in self-construals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90(2), 221–242. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.90.2.221
Hanke, M., Halchenko, Y. O., Sederberg, P. B., Hanson, S. J., Haxby, J. V., & Pollmann, S. (2009). PyMVPA: A python toolbox for multivariate pattern analysis of fMRI data. Neuroinformatics, 7(1), 37–53. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12021-008-9041-y
Herrington, J. D., Mohanty, A., Koven, N. S., Fisher, J. E., Stewart, J. L., Banich, M. T. … Heller, W. (2005). Emotion-modulated performance and activity in left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Emotion, 5(2), 200–207. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.5.2.200
Huang, X., Zhang, H., Chen, C., Xue, G., & He, Q. (2019). The neuroanatomical basis of the Gambler’s fallacy: A univariate and multivariate morphometric study. Human Brain Mapping, 40(3), 967–975. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24425
Iyengar, S. S., Wells, R. E., & Schwartz, B. (2006). Doing better but feeling worse looking for the “Best” job undermines satisfaction. Psychological Science, 17(2), 143–150. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01677.x
Jimura, K., & Poldrack, R. A. (2012). Analyses of regional-average activation and multivoxel pattern information tell complementary stories. Neuropsychologia, 50(4), 544–552. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.11.007
Kawaguchi, A., Nemoto, K., Nakaaki, S., Kawaguchi, T., Kan, H., Arai, N. … Akechi, T. (2016). Insular volume reduction in patients with social anxiety disorder. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 7(JAN), 1–7. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2016.00003
Kriegeskorte, N., Goebel, R., & Bandettini, P. (2006). Information-based functional brain mapping. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103(10), 3863–3868. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0600244103
Kurth, F., Luders, E., & Gaser, C. (2015). Voxel-Based Morphometry. Brain Mapping: An Encyclopedic Reference, 1(December), 345–349. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-397025-1.00304-3
Lai, L. (2011). Maximizing and customer loyalty: Are maximizers less loyal ? 6(4), 307–313
Leach, J. K., & Patall, E. A. (2013). Maximizing and Counterfactual Thinking in Academic Major Decision Making. Journal of Career Assessment, 21(3), 414–429. https://doi.org/10.1177/1069072712475178
Li, S., Shi, F., Pu, F., Li, X., Jiang, T., Xie, S., & Wang, Y. (2007). Hippocampal shape analysis of Alzheimer disease based on machine learning methods. American Journal of Neuroradiology, 28(7), 1339–1345. https://doi.org/10.3174/ajnr.A0620
Lindner, M., Rudorf, S., Birg, R., Falk, A., Weber, B., & Fliessbach, K. (2015). Neural patterns underlying social comparisons of personal performance. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10(4), 569–576. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsu087
Luan, M., & Li, H. (2017). Maximization Paradox Result of Believing in an Objective Best. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 43(5), 652–661. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167217695552
Lv, C., Wang, Q., Chen, C., Xue, G., & He, Q. (2020). Activation patterns of the dorsal medial prefrontal cortex and frontal pole predict individual differences in decision impulsivity. Brain Imaging and Behavior. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-020-00270-1
MacDonald, A. W., Cohen, J. D., Stenger, A., V., & Carter, C. S. (2000). Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. Science, 288(5472), 1835–1838. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.288.5472.1835
Moyano-díaz, E. (2014). The price of gaining: maximization in decision-making, regret and life satisfaction. 9(5), 500–509
Newman, D. B., Schug, J., Yuki, M., Yamada, J., & Nezlek, J. B. (2018). The Negative Consequences of Maximizing in Friendship Selection. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 114(5), 804–824. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000141
Polman, E. (2010). Why are maximizers less happy than satisficers? Because they maximize positive and negative outcomes. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 23(2), 179–190. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.647
Powell, M., & Ansic, D. (1997). Gender differences in risk behaviour in financial decision-making: An experimental analysis. Journal of Economic Psychology, 18(6), 605–628. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0167-4870(97)00026-3
Purvis, A., Howell, R. T., & Iyer, R. (2011). Exploring the role of personality in the relationship between maximization and well-being. Personality and Individual Differences, 50(3), 370–375. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.10.023
Richardson, C. M. E., Jacqueline, H., Ege, E., Suh, H., & Rice, K. G. (2014). Refining the measurement of maximization: Gender invariance and relation to psychological well-being. Personality and Individual Differences, 70, 229–234. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.06.048
Rim, H., & Bin (2017). Impacts of Maximizing Tendencies on Experience-Based Decisions. Psychological Reports, 120(3), 460–474. https://doi.org/10.1177/0033294117697086
Rim, H., Bin, Turner, B. M., Betz, N. E., & Nygren, T. E. (2011). Studies of the dimensionality, correlates, and meaning of measures of the maximizing tendency. Judgment and Decision Making, 6(6), 565–579
Roese, N. J., Pennington, G. L., Coleman, J., Janicki, M., Li, N. P., & Kenrick, D. T. (2006). Sex differences in regret: All for love or some for lust? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32(6), 770–780. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167206286709
Saad, G., & Gill, T. (2001). Sex Differences in the Ultimatum Game: An Evolutionary Psychology Perspective. Journal of Bioeconomics, 3(2/3), 171–193. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1020583425623
Schmidt, U., Friedl, A., & de Lima, K. (2015). Social comparison and gender differences in risk taking. Kiel Institute for the World Economy, 2011
Schwartz, B., Ward, A., Monterosso, J., Lyubomirsky, S., White, K., & Lehman, D. R. (2002). Maximizing versus satisficing: Happiness is a matter of choice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(5), 1178–1197. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.83.5.1178
Simon, H. A. (1955). A Behavioral Model of Rational Choice. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 69(1), 99–118
Simon, H. A. (1956). Rational Choice and the Structure of the Environment. 63(2), 129–138
Sparks, E. A., Ehrlinger, J., & Eibach, R. P. (2012). Failing to commit: Maximizers avoid commitment in a way that contributes to reduced satisfaction. Personality and Individual Differences, 52(1), 72–77. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.09.002
Van Hoeck, N., Ma, N., Ampe, L., Baetens, K., Vandekerckhove, M., & Van Overwalle, F. (2012). Counterfactual thinking: an fMRI study on changing the past for a better future. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(5), 556–564. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nss031
Vargová, L., Zibrínová, Ľ., & Baník, G. (2020). The way of making choices: Maximizing and satisficing and its relationship to well-being, personality, and self-rumination. Judgment and Decision Making, 15(5), 798–806
Verbruggen, F., & Logan, G. D. (2008). Response inhibition in the stop-signal paradigm. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(11), 418–424. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.07.005
von Neumann, J., & Morgenstern, O. (1944). Theory of Games and Economic Behavior
Wang, Q., Chen, C., Cai, Y., Li, S., Zhao, X., Zheng, L. … Xue, G. (2016). Dissociated neural substrates underlying impulsive choice and impulsive action. NeuroImage, 134, 540–549. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.04.010
Wang, Q., Luo, S., Monterosso, J., Zhang, J., Fang, X., Dong, Q., & Xue, G. (2014). Distributed value representation in the medial prefrontal cortex during intertemporal choices. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(22), 7522–7530. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0351-14.2014