Neural efficiency as a function of task demands
Tóm tắt
Từ khóa
Tài liệu tham khảo
Arendasy, 2008, Investigating the g-saturation of various stratum-two factors using automatic item generation, Intelligence, 36, 574, 10.1016/j.intell.2007.11.005
Arendasy, 2008
Arendasy, 2012, Using automatic item generation to meet the increasing item demands of high-stakes assessment, Learning and Individual Differences, 22, 112, 10.1016/j.lindif.2011.11.005
Arendasy, 2012, Gender differences in figural matrices: The moderating role of item design features, Intelligence, 40, 584, 10.1016/j.intell.2012.08.003
Arendasy, 2013, Quantitative differences in retest effects across different methods used to construct alternate test forms, Intelligence, 41, 181, 10.1016/j.intell.2013.02.004
Barbey, 2013, Architecture of fluid intelligence and working memory revealed by lesion mapping, Brain Structure and Function
Basten, 2013, Intelligence is differentially related to neural effort in the task-positive and the task-negative brain network, Intelligence, 41, 517, 10.1016/j.intell.2013.07.006
Brehm, 1989, The intensity of motivation, Annual Review of Psychology, 40, 109, 10.1146/annurev.ps.40.020189.000545
Bressler, 2010, Large-scale brain networks in cognition: Emerging methods and principles, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 277, 10.1016/j.tics.2010.04.004
Carroll, 1993
Cattell, 1971
Colom, 2009, Gray matter correlates of fluid, crystallized, and spatial intelligence: Testing the P-FIT model, Intelligence, 37, 124, 10.1016/j.intell.2008.07.007
Conway, 2002, A latent variable analysis of working memory capacity, short-term memory capacity, processing speed, and general fluid intelligence, Intelligence, 30, 63, 10.1016/S0160-2896(01)00096-4
Deary, 2010, The neuroscience of human intelligence differences, Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 201, 10.1038/nrn2793
Doppelmayr, 2005, Intelligence related differences in EEG bandpower, Neuroscience Letters, 381, 309, 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.02.037
Dunst, 2013, Sex differences in neural efficiency: Are they due to the stereotype threat effect?, Personality and Individual Differences, 55, 744, 10.1016/j.paid.2013.06.007
Gevins, 2000, Neurophysiological measures of working memory and individual differences in cognitive ability and cognitive style, Cerebral Cortex, 10, 829, 10.1093/cercor/10.9.829
Grabner, 2006, Superior performance and neural efficiency: The impact of intelligence and expertise, Brain Research Bulletin, 69, 422, 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2006.02.009
Haier, 1988, Cortical glucose metabolic rate correlates of abstract reasoning and attention studied with positron emission tomography, Intelligence, 12, 199, 10.1016/0160-2896(88)90016-5
Haier, 1992, Intelligence and changes in regional cerebral glucose metabolic rate following learning, Intelligence, 16, 415, 10.1016/0160-2896(92)90018-M
Irvine, 2002, 199
Jaušovec, 2004, Intelligence related differences in induced brain activity during the performance of memory tasks, Personality and Individual Differences, 36, 597, 10.1016/S0191-8869(03)00120-X
Jung, 2007, The parieto-frontal integration theory (P-FIT) of intelligence: Converging neuroimaging evidence, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 135, 10.1017/S0140525X07001185
Kane, 2003, Working-memory capacity and the control of attention: The contributions of goal neglect, response competition, and task set to Stroop interference, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 132, 47, 10.1037/0096-3445.132.1.47
Larson, 1995, Evaluation of a “Mental Effort” hypothesis for correlations between cortical metabolism and intelligence, Intelligence, 21, 267, 10.1016/0160-2896(95)90017-9
Lipp, 2012, Investigating neural efficiency in the visuo-spatial domain: An fMRI study, PLoS One, 7, e51316, 10.1371/journal.pone.0051316
Major, 2011, The dependability of the general factor of intelligence: Why small, single-factor models do not adequately represent g, Intelligence, 39, 418, 10.1016/j.intell.2011.07.002
McGrew, 2009, CHC theory and the human cognitive abilities project: Standing on the shoulders of the giants of psychometric intelligence research, Intelligence, 37, 1, 10.1016/j.intell.2008.08.004
Menon, 2010, Saliency, switching, attention and control: A network model of insula function, Brain Structure and Function, 214, 655, 10.1007/s00429-010-0262-0
Neubauer, 2009, Intelligence and neural efficiency: Measures of brain activation versus measures of functional connectivity in the brain, Intelligence, 37, 223, 10.1016/j.intell.2008.10.008
Neubauer, 2002, Intelligence and neural efficiency: The influence of task content and sex on the brain–IQ relationship, Intelligence, 30, 515, 10.1016/S0160-2896(02)00091-0
Neubauer, 2005, Intelligence and neural efficiency: Further evidence of the influence of task content and sex on the brain–IQ relationship, Cognitive Brain Research, 25, 217, 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.05.011
Neubauer, 2004, Intelligence and individual differences in becoming neurally efficient, Acta Psychologica, 116, 55, 10.1016/j.actpsy.2003.11.005
Neubauer, 1997, Intelligence and reaction times in the Hick, Sternberg and Posner paradigms, Personality and Individual Differences, 22, 885, 10.1016/S0191-8869(97)00003-2
Preusse, 2011, Fluid intelligence allows flexible recruitment of the parieto-frontal network in analogical reasoning, Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5, 1
Rasch, 1980
Rypma, 2002, The influence of working-memory demand and subject performance on prefrontal cortical activity, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 721, 10.1162/08989290260138627
Spearman, 1904, “General intelligence” objectively determined and measured, American Journal of Psychology, 15, 201, 10.2307/1412107
Sridharan, 2008, A critical role for the right fronto-insular cortex in switching between central-executive and default-mode networks, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105, 12569, 10.1073/pnas.0800005105