Neglected attention in apparent spatial compression

Neuropsychologia - Tập 42 - Trang 49-61 - 2004
Paolo Bartolomeo1,2, Marika Urbanski1,3, Sylvie Chokron4,5, Hanna Chainay6, Christine Moroni7, Eric Siéroff3, Catherine Belin8, Peter Halligan9
1French Institute of Biomedical Research, INSERM EMI 007, Centre Paul Broca, 2ter rue d’Alésia, F-75014 Paris, France
2Fédération de Neurologie, Hôpital de la Salpêtrière, Paris, France
3Laboratoire de Psychologie Expérimentale, Université René Descartes (Paris 5), CNRS URA 8581, Paris, France
4Laboratoire de Psychologie Expérimentale, CNRS UMR 5105, Grenoble, France,
5Fondation Ophtalmologique Rothschild, Paris, France
6Université de Bretagne Occidentale, Brest, France
7Laboratoire de Recherche sur l’Evaluation du Comportement et de l’Apprentissage, Université Charles de Gaulle, Lille, France
8Service de Neurologie, CHU Avicenne, AP-HP Paris, Bobigny, France
9 School of Psychology, Cardiff University, Cardiff, UK

Tài liệu tham khảo

Anderson, 1996, A mathematical model of line bisection behaviour in neglect, Brain, 119, 841, 10.1093/brain/119.3.841 Anderson, 2000, Variability not ability: Another basis for performance decrements in neglect, Neuropsychologia, 38, 785, 10.1016/S0028-3932(99)00137-2 Bartolomeo, 1999, Egocentric frame of reference: Its role in spatial bias after right hemisphere lesions, Neuropsychologia, 37, 881, 10.1016/S0028-3932(98)00150-X Bartolomeo, P., & Chokron, S. (2001). Levels of impairment in unilateral neglect. In F. Boller, & J. Grafman (Eds.), Handbook of neuropsychology (Vol. 4, pp. 67–98). Amsterdam: Elsevier. Bartolomeo, 2002, Orienting of attention in left unilateral neglect, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 26, 217, 10.1016/S0149-7634(01)00065-3 Bartolomeo, 1994, The relationship between visuospatial and representational neglect, Neurology, 44, 1710, 10.1212/WNL.44.9.1710 Bartolomeo, 1998, Perception and action in hemispatial neglect, Neuropsychologia, 36, 227, 10.1016/S0028-3932(97)00104-8 Bartolomeo, 2001, Variability of response times as a marker of diverted attention, Neuropsychologia, 39, 358, 10.1016/S0028-3932(00)00135-4 Barton, 1998, Line bisection in hemianopia, Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 64, 660, 10.1136/jnnp.64.5.660 Bisiach, 1993, Mental representation in unilateral neglect and related disorders, The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46A, 435, 10.1080/14640749308401056 Bisiach, 1983, Line bisection and cognitive plasticity of unilateral neglect of space, Brain and Cognition, 2, 32, 10.1016/0278-2626(83)90027-1 Bisiach, 1981, Brain and conscious representation of outside reality, Neuropsychologia, 19, 543, 10.1016/0028-3932(81)90020-8 Bisiach, 1978, Unilateral neglect of representational space, Cortex, 14, 129, 10.1016/S0010-9452(78)80016-1 Bisiach, 1996, Beyond unilateral neglect, Brain, 119, 851, 10.1093/brain/119.3.851 Butter, 1988, Changes in sensory inattention, Neuropsychologia, 26, 533, 10.1016/0028-3932(88)90110-8 Chatterjee, A. (2002). Spatial anisometry and representational release in neglect. In H. O. Karnath, D. Milner, & G. Vallar (Eds.), The cognitive and neural bases of spatial neglect (pp. 167–180). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Chatterjee, 1999, Quantitative analysis of cancellation tasks in neglect, Cortex, 35, 253, 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70798-6 Chokron, 1997, Length representation in normal and neglect subjects with opposite reading habits studied through a line extension task, Cortex, 33, 47, 10.1016/S0010-9452(97)80004-4 Chokron, S., Colliot, P., Bartolomeo, P. The role of vision on spatial representations. Cortex, in press. Daini, 2002, Exploring the syndrome of spatial unilateral neglect through an illusion of length, Experimental Brain Research, 144, 224, 10.1007/s00221-002-1034-8 De Renzi, 1989, Attentional shifts toward the rightmost stimuli in patients with left visual neglect, Cortex, 25, 231, 10.1016/S0010-9452(89)80039-5 D’Erme, 1987, Influenza dell’emi-inattenzione e dell’emianopsia sui compiti di bisezione di linee nei pazienti cerebrolesi, Archivio di Psicologia, Neurologia e Psichiatria, 48, 165 D’Erme, 1992, Early rightwards orienting of attention on simple reaction time performance in patients with left-sided neglect, Neuropsychologia, 30, 989, 10.1016/0028-3932(92)90050-V Desimone, 1995, Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention, Annual Review in Neurosciences, 18, 193, 10.1146/annurev.ne.18.030195.001205 Doricchi, 1999, Misrepresentation of horizontal space in left unilateral neglect: Role of hemianopia, Neurology, 52, 1845, 10.1212/WNL.52.9.1845 Driver, J., & Husain, M. (2002). The role of spatial working memory deficits in pathological search by neglect patients. In H. O. Karnath, D. Milner, & G. Vallar (Eds.), The cognitive and neural bases of spatial neglect (pp. 351–62). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Fuchs, 1920, Untersuchung über das Sehen der Hemianopiker und Hemiamblyopiker, Zeitschrift für Psychologie und Physiologie Sinnersorganen, 84, 67 Gainotti, 1991, Early orientation of attention toward the half space ipsilateral to the lesion in patients with unilateral brain damage, Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 54, 1082, 10.1136/jnnp.54.12.1082 Gauthier, 1989, The bells test: A quantitative and qualitative test for visual neglect, International Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology, 11, 49 Halligan, 1989, Is neglect (only) lateral? A quadrant analysis of line cancellation, Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 11, 793, 10.1080/01688638908400936 Halligan, 1991, Spatial compression in visual neglect: A case study, Cortex, 27, 623, 10.1016/S0010-9452(13)80011-1 Halligan, 1998, Visuospatial neglect: The ultimate deconstruction?, Brain and Cognition, 37, 419, 10.1006/brcg.1998.1006 Halligan, 1990, Do visual field deficits exacerbate visuo-spatial neglect?, Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 53, 487, 10.1136/jnnp.53.6.487 Heilman, 1985, Directional hypokinesia: Prolonged reaction times for leftward movements in patients with right hemisphere lesions and neglect, Neurology, 35, 855, 10.1212/WNL.35.6.855 Heilman, K. M., Watson, R. T., & Valenstein, E. (2002). Spatial neglect. In H. O. Karnath, D. Milner, & G. Vallar (Eds.), The cognitive and neural bases of spatial neglect (pp. 3–30). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Hoffman, 1995, The role of visual attention in saccadic eye movements, Perception and Psychophysics, 57, 787, 10.3758/BF03206794 Kerkhoff, 2000, Spatial hemineglect in humans, Progress in Neurobiology, 63, 1, 10.1016/S0301-0082(00)00028-9 Kinsbourne, 1970, A model for the mechanism of unilateral neglect of space, Transactions of the American Neurological Association, 95, 143 Kinsbourne, M. (1993). Orientational bias model of unilateral neglect: Evidence from attentional gradients within hemispace. In I. H. Robertson, & J. C. Marshall (Eds.), Unilateral neglect: Clinical and experimental studies (pp. 63–86). Hove (UK): Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Kowler, 1995, The role of attention in the programming of saccades, Vision Research, 35, 1897, 10.1016/0042-6989(94)00279-U Mark, 1997, Diagonal neglect on cancellation, Neuropsychologia, 35, 1425, 10.1016/S0028-3932(97)00067-5 Mark, 1988, Hemispatial neglect affected by non-neglected stimuli, Neurology, 38, 640, 10.1212/WNL.38.8.1207 Marshall, 1989, Does the midsagittal plane play any privileged role in “left” neglect?, Cognitive Neuropsychology, 6, 403, 10.1080/02643298908253290 Mattingley, 1992, Impairments of movement initiation and execution in unilateral neglect, Brain, 115, 1849, 10.1093/brain/115.6.1849 Mattingley, 1998, Motor role of human inferior parietal lobe revealed in unilateral neglect patients, Nature, 392, 179, 10.1038/32413 Milner, A. D. (1987). Animal models for the syndrome of spatial neglect. In M. Jeannerod (Ed.), Neurophysiological and neuropsychological aspects of spatial neglect (pp. 148–77). Amsterdam: Elsevier. Milner, 1995, Distortion of size perception in visuospatial neglect, Current Biology, 5, 85, 10.1016/S0960-9822(95)00020-0 Mozer, 1997, The end of the line for a brain-damaged model of unilateral neglect, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 171, 10.1162/jocn.1997.9.2.171 Posner, 1984, Effects of parietal injury on covert orienting of attention, Journal of Neuroscience, 4, 1863, 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.04-07-01863.1984 Previc, 1990, Functional specialization in the lower and upper visual fields in humans: Its ecological origins and neurophysiological implications, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 13, 519, 10.1017/S0140525X00080018 Prinzmetal, 1998, The phenomenology of attention. Part 1. Color, location, orientation, and “clarity”, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 261 Prinzmetal, 1997, The phenomenology of attention. Part 2. Brightness and contrast, Consciousness and Cognition, 6, 372, 10.1006/ccog.1997.0313 Prinzmetal, 1997, The effect of attention on phenomenal length, Perception, 26, 193, 10.1068/p260193 Ricci, 2000, Orientation bias in unilateral neglect: Representational contributions, Cortex, 36, 671, 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70544-6 Rousseaux, 2001, Normalisation d’une batterie de dépistage de la négligence spatiale. Etude de l’effet de l’âge, du niveau d’éducation, du sexe, de la main et de la latéralité [Presenting a battery for assessing spatial neglect. Norms and effects of age, educational level, sex, hand and laterality], Revue Neurologique, 157, 1385 Shepherd, 1986, The relationship between eye movements and spatial attention, The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38A, 475, 10.1080/14640748608401609 Vallar, 1998, Spatial hemineglect in humans, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 87, 10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01145-0 Werth, 1988, Compression and lateral shift of mental coordinate systems in a line bisection task, Neuropsychologia, 26, 741, 10.1016/0028-3932(88)90010-3