Mental files: Developmental integration of dual naming and theory of mind
Tài liệu tham khảo
Akhtar, 2012, Cognitive and linguistic correlates of early exposure to more than one language, Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 42, 41, 10.1016/B978-0-12-394388-0.00002-2
Apperly, 1998, Children's mental representation of referential relations, Cognition, 67, 287, 10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00030-4
Au, 1990, The principle of mutual exclusivity in word learning. To honour or not to honour?, Child Development, 61, 1474, 10.2307/1130757
Baillargeon, 2010, False-belief understanding in infants, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 110, 10.1016/j.tics.2009.12.006
Bloom, 2000
Bosco, 2006, Recognition and repair of communicative failures: A developmental perspective, Journal of Pragmatics, 38, 1398, 10.1016/j.pragma.2005.06.011
Brennan, 1996, Conceptual pacts and lexical choice in conversation, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 22, 1482
Butterfill, 2013, How to construct a minimal theory of mind, Mind & Language, 28, 606, 10.1111/mila.12036
Byers-Heinlein, 2009, Monolingual, bilingual, trilingual: Infants' language experience influences the development of a word-learning heuristic, Developmental Science, 12, 815, 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00902.x
Byers-Heinlein, 2013, Lexicon structure and the disambiguation of novel words: Evidence from bilingual infants, Cognition, 128, 407, 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.05.010
Carey, 1978, Acquiring a single new word, Papers and Reports on Child Language Development, 15, 17
Carruthers, 2013, Mindreading in infancy, Mind & Language, 28, 141, 10.1111/mila.12014
Clark, 1987, The principle of contrast: A constraint on language acquisition, 1
Clark, 1997, Conceptual perspective and lexical choice in acquisition, Cognition, 64, 1, 10.1016/S0010-0277(97)00010-3
Clark, 1997, Speaker perspective and reference in young children, First Language, 17, 57, 10.1177/014272379701704903
Clements, 1994, Implicit understanding of belief, Cognitive Development, 9, 377, 10.1016/0885-2014(94)90012-4
Davidson, 1997, Monolingual and bilingual children's use of the mutual exclusivity constraint, Journal of Child Language, 24, 3, 10.1017/S0305000996002917
Davidson, 2005, Monolingual and bilingual children’s use of mutual exclusivity in the naming of whole objects, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92, 25, 10.1016/j.jecp.2005.03.007
de Marchena, 2011, Mutual exclusivity in autism spectrum disorders: Testing the pragmatic hypothesis, Cognition, 119, 96, 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.12.011
Deák, 1998, On having complex representations of things: Preschoolers use multiple words for objects and people, Developmental Psychology, 34, 224, 10.1037/0012-1649.34.2.224
Diaz, 2018, The missing explanation of the false-belief advantage in bilingual children: A longitudinal study, Developmental Science, 21, e12594, 10.1111/desc.12594
Diesendruck, 2001, Children's avoidance of lexical overlap: A pragmatic account, Developmental Psychology, 37, 630, 10.1037/0012-1649.37.5.630
Dockrell, 1981
Dockrell, 1986, Lexical acquisition strategies in the preschool child
Doherty, 2000, Children's understanding of homonymy: Metalinguistic awareness and false belief, Journal of Child Language, 27, 367, 10.1017/S0305000900004153
Doherty, 2006, The development of mentalistic gaze understanding, Infant and Child Development, 15, 179, 10.1002/icd.434
Doherty, 1998, Metalinguistic awareness and theory of mind: Just two words for the same thing?, Cognitive Development, 13, 279, 10.1016/S0885-2014(98)90012-0
Doherty, 2005, Children's understanding of ambiguous figures: Which cognitive developments are necessary to experience reversal?, Cognitive Development, 20, 407, 10.1016/j.cogdev.2005.05.003
Fagan, 2008, Toddlers' persistence when communication fails: Response motivation and goal substitution, First Language, 28, 55, 10.1177/0142723707083280
Farrar, 2012, Phonological awareness, executive functioning, and theory of mind, Cognitive Development, 27, 77, 10.1016/j.cogdev.2011.08.002
Flavell, 1988, The development of children's knowledge about the mind: From cognitive connections to mental representations, 244
Flavell, 1981, Young children’s knowledge about visual perception: Further evidence for the Level 1–Level 2 distinction, Developmental Psychology, 17, 99, 10.1037/0012-1649.17.1.99
Flavell, 1983, Development of the appearance-reality distinction, Cognitive Psychology, 15, 95, 10.1016/0010-0285(83)90005-1
Frank, 2002, Young monolingual and bilingual children's responses to violation of the mutual exclusivity principle, The International Journal of Bilingualism, 6, 125, 10.1177/13670069020060020201
Frege, 1970, On Sense and Reference (1892), translated by Max Black
Genesee, 1996, Talking with strangers: A study of bilingual children's communicative competence, Applied Psycholinguistics, 17, 427, 10.1017/S0142716400008183
Goetz, 2003, The effects of bilingualism on theory of mind development, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 6, 1, 10.1017/S1366728903001007
Golinkoff, 1986, ‘I beg your pardon?’: The preverbal negotiation of failed messages, Journal of Child Language, 13, 455, 10.1017/S0305000900006826
Golinkoff, 1993, When is communication a ‘meeting of minds’?, Journal of Child Language, 20, 199, 10.1017/S030500090000920X
Golinkoff, 1992, Young children and adults use lexical principles to learn new nouns, Developmental Psychology, 28, 99, 10.1037/0012-1649.28.1.99
Gollek, C. (2013). Metacognitive development and the disambiguation effect in monolingual and bilingual children. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, School of Natural Sciences, University of Stirling.
Gollek, 2016, Metacognitive developments in word learning: Mutual exclusivity and theory of mind, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 148, 51, 10.1016/j.jecp.2016.03.007
Gopnik, 1988
Gopnik, 2001, Duck or rabbit? Reversing ambiguous figures and understanding ambiguous representations, Developmental Science, 4, 175, 10.1111/1467-7687.00163
Grassmann, 2015, Children’s level of word knowledge predicts their exclusion of familiar objects as referents of novel words, Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1200, 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01200
Halberda, 2003, The development of a word-learning strategy, Cognition, 87, B23, 10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00186-5
Hamilton, 2009, Visual perspective taking impairment in children with autistic spectrum disorder, Cognition, 113, 37, 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.07.007
Haring, S. (2013). Probleme mit Identitätsaussagen im Kindergartenalter. Unpublished Diploma Thesis, Department of Psychology, University of Salzburg.
Haryu, 1991, Continuity and the persistence of objects: When the whole is greater than the sum of the parts, Japanese Journal of Educational Psychology, 39, 11, 10.5926/jjep1953.39.1_11
Haryu, 1999, Controlling the application of mutual exclusivity assumption in the acquisition of lexical hierarchies, Japanese Psychological Research, 41, 21, 10.1111/1468-5884.00102
Helming, 2016, Solving the puzzle about early belief-ascription, Mind & Language, 31, 438, 10.1111/mila.12114
Houston-Price, 2010, Language experience shapes the development of the mutual exclusivity bias, Infancy, 15, 125, 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2009.00009.x
Huemer, M., Haslehner, T. & Perner, J. (2019). “The development of conceptual perspective taking.“ Poster presented at the Biennial Meeting of SRCD, 21 – 23 March 2019, Baltimore, Maryland.
Kahneman, 1992, The reviewing of object files: Object-specific integration of information, Cognitive psychology, 24, 175, 10.1016/0010-0285(92)90007-O
Kalashnikova, 2015, The effects of linguistic experience on the flexible use of mutual exclusivity in word learning, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18, 626, 10.1017/S1366728914000364
Kalashnikova, 2018, Acceptance of lexical overlap by monolingual and bilingual toddlers, International Journal of Bilingualism, 1367006918808041
Karadaki, T. (2019). Children’s referent selection and theory of mind. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, School of Psychology, University of East Anglia.
Karadaki, T., & Doherty, M.J. (2017) Preschool metacognitive developments allow learning and use of co-referential words. Poster presented at the IX. Dubrovnik Conference on Cognitive Science, Dubrovnik, Croatia, May 2017.
Karttunen, L. (1976). Discourse referents. In J. McCawley (Ed.), Notes from the linguistic underground (Syntax and Semantics, vol. 7) (pp. 363–385).
Knudsen, 2013, One-year-olds warn others about negative action outcomes, Journal of Cognition and Development, 14, 424, 10.1080/15248372.2012.689387
Kovács, 2009, Early bilingualism enhances mechanisms of false-belief reasoning, Developmental Science, 12, 48, 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00742.x
Kronmüller, 2015, Referential precedents in spoken language comprehension: A review and meta-analysis, Journal of Memory and Language, 83, 1, 10.1016/j.jml.2015.03.008
Kulke, 2018, Implicit theory of mind – An overview of current replications and non-replications, Data in Brief, 16, 101, 10.1016/j.dib.2017.11.016
Leslie, 2005, Belief-desire reasoning as a process of selection, Cognitive Psychology, 50, 45, 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2004.06.002
Leslie, 1998, Inhibitory processing in the false belief task: Two conjectures, Developmental Science, 1, 247, 10.1111/1467-7687.00038
Lempers, 1977, The development in very young children of tacit knowledge concerning visual perception, Genetic Psychology Monographs, 95, 3
Lewis, M., Cristiano, V., Lake, B. M., Kwan, T., & Frank, M. C. (2019). The role of developmental change and linguistic experience in the mutual exclusivity effect. psyarxiv.com.
Lockwood, 1971, Identity and Reference, 199
Malone, 2016, Is it a name or a fact? Disambiguation of reference via exclusivity and pragmatic reasoning, Cognitive Science, 40, 2095, 10.1111/cogs.12321
Mani, 2010, In the infant’s mind’s ear: Evidence for implicit naming in 18-month-olds, Psychological Science, 21, 908, 10.1177/0956797610373371
Markman, 1989
Markman, 1988, Children's use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meanings of words, Cognitive Psychology, 20, 121, 10.1016/0010-0285(88)90017-5
Markman, 2003, Use of the mutual exclusivity assumption by young word learners, Cognitive Psychology, 47, 241, 10.1016/S0010-0285(03)00034-3
Masangkay, 1974, The early development of inferences about the visual percepts of others, Child Development, 357, 10.2307/1127956
Matthews, 2010, What's in a manner of speaking? Children's sensitivity to partner-specific referential precedents, Developmental Psychology, 46, 749, 10.1037/a0019657
McGregor, 1998, Object naming at multiple hierarchical levels: A comparison of preschoolers with and without word-finding deficits, Journal of Child Language, 25, 419, 10.1017/S030500099800347X
Meitner, L. (2008). Verständnis von Identitätsaussagen im Vorschulalter. Unpublished Masters thesis. Department of Psychology, University of Salzburg.
Merriman, W. E., & Bowman, L. L. (1989). The mutual exclusivity bias in children's word learning. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 54 (3-4, Serial No. 220).
Metzing, 2003, When conceptual pacts are broken: Partner-specific effects on the comprehension of referring expressions, Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 201, 10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00028-7
Moll, 2007, How 14-and 18-month-olds know what others have experienced, Developmental Psychology, 43, 309, 10.1037/0012-1649.43.2.309
Murez, 2016, Mental Files: An Introduction, Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 7, 265, 10.1007/s13164-016-0314-3
Neumann, A., Thoermer, C., & Sodian, B. (2008). False belief understanding in 18‐month‐olds’ anticipatory looking behavior: An eye‐tracking study. Paper presented at the XXIX International Congress of Psychology, Berlin, Germany.
Nguyen, T-K. (2011). Cognitive abilities underlying the bilingual advantage in set shifting. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Department of Human Development and Applied Psychology, University of Toronto, Canada.
Nguyen, 2014, Reassessing the bilingual advantage in theory of mind and its cognitive underpinnings, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17, 396, 10.1017/S1366728913000394
O'Neill, 1996, Two-year-old children's sensitivity to a parent's knowledge state when making requests, Child Development, 67, 659, 10.2307/1131839
Pearson, D.K. (2013). Effect of Language Background on Metalinguistic Awareness and Theory of Mind. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, School of Natural Sciences, University of Stirling.
Perner, J. (2016). Referential and cooperative bias: In defence of an implicit theory of mind. Brains Blog. Commentary in symposium on Katharina Helming, Brent Strickland, and Pierre Jacob's “Solving the puzzle about early belief-ascription”. http://philosophyofbrains.com/2016/10/17/symposium-on-helming-strickland-and-jacob-solving-the- puzzle-about-early-belief-ascription.aspx.
Onishi, 2005, Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs?, Science, 308, 255, 10.1126/science.1107621
Perner, 2005, File change semantics for preschoolers: Alternative naming and belief understanding, Interaction Studies, 6, 483, 10.1075/is.6.3.11per
Perner, 2016, Mental files in development: Dual naming, false belief, identity and intensionality, Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 7, 491, 10.1007/s13164-015-0235-6
Perner, 2011, Identity: Key to children’s understanding of belief, Science, 333, 474, 10.1126/science.1201216
Perner, 2007, Objects of desire, thought, and reality: Problems of anchoring discourse referents in development, Mind & Language, 22, 475, 10.1111/j.1468-0017.2007.00317.x
Perner, 2012, From infants’ to children's appreciation of belief, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 519, 10.1016/j.tics.2012.08.004
Perner, 2002, Theory of mind finds its Piagetian perspective: Why alternative naming comes with understanding belief, Cognitive Development, 17, 1451, 10.1016/S0885-2014(02)00127-2
Perry, 2002
Pylyshyn, 2000, Situating vision in the world, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 197, 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01477-7
Pylyshyn, 2007
Rakoczy, 2015, Explicit theory of mind is even more unified than previously assumed: Belief ascription and understanding aspectuality emerge together in development, Child Development, 86, 486, 10.1111/cdev.12311
Recanati, 2012
Rubio-Fernández, 2013, How to pass the false-belief task before your fourth birthday, Psychological Science, 24, 27, 10.1177/0956797612447819
Rubio-Fernández, 2016, Don’t mention the marble! The role of attentional processes in false-belief tasks, Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 7, 835, 10.1007/s13164-015-0290-z
Ruffman, 2001, Does eye gaze indicate implicit knowledge of false belief? Charting transitions in knowledge, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 80, 201, 10.1006/jecp.2001.2633
Russell, 1996
Savage, 1996, What word learners do when input contradicts the mutual exclusivity assumption, Child Development, 67, 3120, 10.2307/1131770
Scholl, 1999, Explaining the infant’s object concept: Beyond the perception/cognition dichotomy, 26
Schroeder, 2018, Do bilinguals have an advantage in theory of mind? A meta-analysis, Frontiers in Communication, 3, 36, 10.3389/fcomm.2018.00036
Scofield, 2007, Two-year-olds differentially disambiguate novel words and facts, Journal of Child Language, 34, 889
Setoh, 2016, Two-and-a-half-year-olds succeed at a traditional false-belief task with reduced processing demands, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113, 13360, 10.1073/pnas.1609203113
Shatz, 1990, Conversational or communicative skill? A reassessment of two-year-olds' behaviour in miscommunication episodes, Journal of Child Language, 17, 131, 10.1017/S0305000900013143
Sodian, 2007, Now I see it but you don't: 14-month-olds can represent another person's visual perspective, Developmental Science, 10, 199, 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00580.x
Southgate, 2019, Are infants altercentric? The other and the self in early social cognition, Psychological Review, 10.1037/rev0000182
Southgate, 2007, Action anticipation through attribution of false belief by 2-year-olds, Psychological Science, 18, 587, 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01944.x
Strawson, 1974
Surian, 2007, Attribution of beliefs by 13-month-old infants, Psychological Science, 18, 580, 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01943.x
Tare, 2010, Can you say it another way? Cognitive factors in bilingual children's pragmatic language skills, Journal of Cognition and Development, 11, 137, 10.1080/15248371003699951
Thoermer, 2012, Continuity from an implicit to an explicit understanding of false belief from infancy to preschool age, British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30, 172, 10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02067.x
Tomasello, 1999
Tomasello, 2000, The social-pragmatic theory of word learning, Pragmatics, 10, 401, 10.1075/prag.10.4.01tom
Tomasello, 2003, Understanding attention: 12-and 18-month-olds know what is new for other persons, Developmental Psychology, 39, 906, 10.1037/0012-1649.39.5.906
Treisman, 1982, Perceptual grouping and attention in visual search for features and for objects, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 8, 194
Waidmann, G. (2006). Theory of Mind und Identitätsverständnis im Vorschulalter. Unpublished Masterthesis, Department of Psychology, University of Salzburg.
Waxman, 1992, Beyond the basics: Preschool children label objects flexibly at multiple hierarchical levels, Journal of Child Language, 19, 153, 10.1017/S0305000900013672
Wellman, 2001, Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief, Child Development, 72, 655, 10.1111/1467-8624.00304
Wellman, 2004, Scaling of theory-of-mind tasks, Child Development, 75, 523, 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00691.x
Westra, 2017, Stereotypes, theory of mind, and the action–prediction hierarchy, Synthese, 1
Wimmer, 1983, Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception, Cognition, 13, 103, 10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5
Wimmer, 2011, The development of ambiguous figure perception, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 76, vii
Xu, 1996, Infants’ metaphysics: The case of numerical identity, Cognitive Psychology, 30, 111, 10.1006/cogp.1996.0005