Mental Time Travel? A Neurocognitive Model of Event Simulation

Springer Science and Business Media LLC - Tập 11 Số 2 - Trang 233-259 - 2020
Donna Rose Addis1
1Rotman Research Institute, Baycrest Health Sciences, Toronto, Canada

Tóm tắt

Từ khóa


Tài liệu tham khảo

Addis, D.R. 2018. Are episodic memories special? On the sameness of remembered and imagined event simulation. Journal of the Royal Society of New Zealand 48 (2–3): 64–88. https://doi.org/10.1080/03036758.2018.1439071.

Addis, D.R., T. Cheng, R.P. Roberts, and D.L. Schacter. 2011. Hippocampal contributions to the episodic simulation of specific and general future events. Hippocampus 21: 1045–1052. https://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.20870.

Addis, D.R., S. Hach, and L.J. Tippett. 2016. Do strategic processes contribute to the specificity of future simulation in depression? The British Journal of Clinical Psychology 55: 167–186. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjc.12103.

Addis, D.R., M. Moscovitch, A.P. Crawley, and M.P. McAndrews. 2004. Recollective qualities modulate hippocampal activation during autobiographical memory retrieval. Hippocampus 14: 752–762. https://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.10215.

Addis, D.R., L. Pan, M.A. Vu, N. Laiser, and D.L. Schacter. 2009. Constructive episodic simulation of the future and the past: Distinct subsystems of a core brain network mediate imagining and remembering. Neuropsychologia 47: 2222–2238. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.10.026.

Addis, D.R., and D.L. Schacter. 2012. The Hippocampus and Imagining the Future: Where Do We Stand? Front Hum Neurosci 5: 173. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2011.00173.

Addis, D.R., A.T. Wong, and D.L. Schacter. 2007. Remembering the past and imagining the future: Common and distinct neural substrates during event construction and elaboration. Neuropsychologia 45: 1363–1377. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.10.016.

Addis, D.R., A.T. Wong, and D.L. Schacter. 2008. Age-related changes in the episodic simulation of future events. Psychological Science 19: 33. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02043.x.

Anderson, R.J., S.A. Dewhurst, and R.A. Nash. 2012. Shared cognitive processes underlying past and future thinking: the impact of imagery and concurrent task demands on event specificity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition 38: 356–365. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025451.

Arzy, S., and D.L. Schacter. 2019. Self-agency and self-ownership in cognitive mapping. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 23: 476–487. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2019.04.003.

Atance, C.M., and D.K. O'Neill. 2001. Episodic future thinking. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 5 (12): 533–539. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01804-0.

Baddeley, A.D. 2000. The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4: 417–423. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01538-2.

Baldassano, C., J. Chen, A. Zadbood, J.W. Pillow, U. Hasson, and K.A. Norman. 2017. Discovering event structure in continuous narrative perception and memory. Neuron 95: 709–721. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2017.06.041.

Bar, M. 2009. The proactive brain: Memory for predictions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences 364 (1521): 1235–1243. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0310.

Bartlett, F.C. 1932. Remembering. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Beaty, R.E., M. Benedek, S. Barry Kaufman, and P.J. Silvia. 2015. Default and executive network coupling supports creative idea production. Scientific Reports 5: 10964. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep10964.

Behrens, T.E.J., T.H. Muller, J.C.R. Whittington, S. Mark, A.B. Baram, K.L. Stachenfeld, and Z. Kurth-Nelson. 2018. What is a cognitive map? Organizing knowledge for flexible behavior. Neuron 100: 490–509. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2018.10.002.

Binder, J.R., R.H. Desai, W.W. Graves, and L.L. Conant. 2009. Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. Cerebral Cortex 19: 2767–2796. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhp055.

Brewin, C.R. 2014. Episodic memory, perceptual memory, and their interaction: Foundations for a theory of posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychological Bulletin 140: 69–97. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033722.

Buckner, R.L., and D.C. Carroll. 2007. Self-projection and the brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 11: 49–57. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.004.

Burianova, H., and C.L. Grady. 2007. Common and unique neural activations in autobiographical, episodic, and semantic retrieval. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 19: 1520–1534. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2007.19.9.1520.

Cabeza, R., and M. Moscovitch. 2013. Memory systems, processing modes, and components: Functional neuroimaging evidence. Perspectives on Psychological Science 8: 49–55. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691612469033.

Cabral, J., M.L. Kringelbach, and G. Deco. 2014. Exploring the network dynamics underlying brain activity during rest. Progress in Neurobiology 114: 102–131. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pneurobio.2013.12.005.

Chen, J., C.J. Honey, E. Simony, M.J. Arcaro, K.A. Norman, and U. Hasson. 2016. Accessing real-life episodic information from minutes versus hours earlier modulates hippocampal and high-order cortical dynamics. Cerebral Cortex 26: 3428–3441. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhv155.

Christoff, K., Z.C. Irving, K.C.R. Fox, R.N. Spreng, and J.R. Andrews-Hanna. 2016. Mind-wandering as spontaneous thought: A dynamic framework. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 17: 718–731. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2016.113.

Christoff, K., V. Prabhakaran, J. Dorfman, Z. Zhao, J.K. Kroger, K.J. Holyoak, and J.D. Gabrieli. 2001. Rostrolateral prefrontal cortex involvement in relational integration during reasoning. Neuroimage 14 (5): 1136–1149. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2001.0922.

Conway, M.A., and C.W. Pleydell-Pearce. 2000. The construction of autobiographical memories in the self-memory system. Psychological Review 107: 261–288. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.107.2.261.

Craik, F.I.M. 2007. Encoding: A cognitive perspective. In Science of memory concepts, ed. H.L. Roediger, Y. Dudai, and S.M. Fitzpatrick, 129–136. New York: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195310443.003.0007.

Craik, F.I.M., and R.S. Lockhart. 1972. Levels of processing: A framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 11: 671–684. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(72)80001-X.

D'Argembeau, A., D. Stawarczyk, S. Majerus, F. Collette, M. Van der Linden, D. Feyers, et al. 2010. The neural basis of personal goal processing when envisioning future events. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 22: 1701–1713. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21314.

D'Argembeau, A., and M. Van der Linden. 2004. Phenomenal characteristics associated with projecting oneself back into the past and forward into the future: Influence of valence and temporal distance. Consciousness & Cognition 13: 844–858.

D'Argembeau, A., and M. Van der Linden. 2006. Individuals differences in the phenomenology of mental time travel: The effects of vivid visual imagery and emotion regulation strategies. Consciousness and Cognition 15: 342–350. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2004.07.007.

D'Argembeau, A., and M. Van der Linden. 2012. Predicting the phenomenology of episodic future thoughts. Consciousness and Cognition 21: 1198–1206. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2012.05.004.

Dannenberg, H., A.S. Alexander, J.C. Robinson, and M.E. Hasselmo. 2018. The role of hierarchical dynamical functions in coding for episodic memory and cognition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 31 (9): 1–19. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01439.

Dayan, P., G.E. Hinton, and R.M.T. Neal. 1995. The Helmholtz machine. Neural Computation 7: 889–904. https://doi.org/10.1162/neco.1995.7.5.889.

De Brigard, F., D.R. Addis, J.H. Ford, D.L. Schacter, and K.S. Giovanello. 2013. Remembering what could have happened: Neural correlates of episodic counterfactual thinking. Neuropsychologia 51: 2401–2414. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.01.015.

de Vito, S., N. Gamboz, and M.A. Brandimonte. 2012a. What differentiates episodic future thinking from complex scene imagery? Consciousness and Cognition 21: 813–823. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2012.01.013.

de Vito, S., N. Gamboz, A.M. Brandimonte, P. Barone, M. Amboni, and S. Della Sala. 2012b. Future thinking in Parkinson's disease: An executive function? Neuropsychologia 50: 1494–1501. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.03.001.

Devitt, A.L., E. Monk-Fromont, D.L. Schacter, and D.R. Addis. 2016. Factors that influence the generation of autobiographical memory conjunction errors. Memory 24: 204–222. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2014.998680.

Dixon, M.L., A. De La Vega, C. Mills, J.R. Andrews-Hanna, R.N. Spreng, M.W. Cole, and K. Christoff. 2018. Heterogeneity within the frontoparietal control network and its relationship to the default and dorsal attention networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 115: E1598–E1607. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1715766115.

Ernst, A., A. Scoboria, and A. D’Argembeau. 2019. On the role of autobiographical knowledge in shaping belief in the future occurrence of imagined events. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 72: 2658–2671. https://doi.org/10.1177/1747021819855621.

Frith, U., and C. Frith. 2003. Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences 358: 459–473. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2002.1218.

Fuster, J.M. 1997. Network memory. Trends in Neurosciences 20: 451–459. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0166-2236(97)01128-4.

Garry, M., C.G. Manning, E.F. Loftus, and S.J. Sherman. 1996. Imagination inflation: Imagining a childhood event inflates confidence that it occurred. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 3 (2): 208–214. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03212420.

Gentner, D. 1983. Structure-mapping: A theoretical framework for analogy. Cognitive Science 7: 155–170. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0364-0213(83)80009-3.

Ghosh, V.E., and A. Gilboa. 2014. What is a memory schema? A historical perspective on current neuroscience literature. Neuropsychologia 53: 104–114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.11.010.

Gilboa, A., and H. Marlatte. 2017. Neurobiology of schemas and schema-mediated memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 21: 618–631. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2017.04.013.

Gilboa, A., and M. Moscovitch. 2017. Ventromedial prefrontal cortex generates pre-stimulus theta coherence desynchronization: A schema instantiation hypothesis. Cortex 87: 16–30. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2016.10.008.

Graf, P., and D.L. Schacter. 1989. Unitization and grouping mediate dissociations in memory for new associations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition 15: 930–940. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.15.5.930.

Grysman, A., J. Prabhakar, S.M. Anglin, and J.A. Hudson. 2013. The time travelling self: Comparing self and other in narratives of past and future events. Consciousness & Cognition 22: 742–755. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2013.04.010.

Hassabis, D., D. Kumaran, S.D. Vann, and E.A. Maguire. 2007. Patients with hippocampal amnesia cannot imagine new experiences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 104: 1726–1731. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0610561104.

Hassabis, D., and E.A. Maguire. 2007. Deconstructing episodic memory with construction. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 11: 299–306.

Hasson, U., J. Chen, and C.J. Honey. 2015. Hierarchical process memory: Memory as an integral component of information processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 19: 304–313. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.04.006.

Hebb, D.O. 1949. The organization of behavior: A neuropsychological theory. Oxford: Wiley.

Holyoak, K.J. 2012. Analogy and relational reasoning. In The Oxford handbook of thinking and reasoning, ed. K.J. Holyoak and R.G. Morrison, 234–259. New York, N.Y: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199734689.013.0013.

Horner, A.J., and N. Burgess. 2013. The associative structure of memory for multi-element events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 142: 1370–1383. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033626.

Hummel, J.E., and K.J. Holyoak. 2003. A symbolic-connectionist theory of relational inference and generalization. Psychological Review 110: 220–263. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.110.2.220.

Irish, M., D.R. Addis, J. Hodges, and O. Piguet. 2012a. Considering the role of semantic memory in episodic future thinking: Evidence from semantic dementia. Brain 135: 2178–2191. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/aws119.

Irish, M., D.R. Addis, J. Hodges, and O. Piguet. 2012b. Exploring the content and quality of episodic future thinking simulations in semantic dementia. Neuropsychologia 50: 3488–3495. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.09.012.

Irish, M., and O. Piguet. 2013. The pivotal role of semantic memory in remembering the past and imagining the future. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 7: 27. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00027.

Isomura, T., T. Parr, and K. Friston. 2019. Bayesian filtering with multiple internal models: Toward a theory of social intelligence. Neural Computation 31: 2390–2431. https://doi.org/10.1162/neco_a_01239.

Johnson, M.K., S. Hashtroudi, and D.S. Lindsay. 1993. Source monitoring. Psychological Bulletin 114: 3–28. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.114.1.3.

Johnson, M.K., and C.L. Raye. 1981. Reality monitoring. Psychological Review 88: 67–85. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.88.1.67.

Kensinger, E.A., R.J. Garoff-Eaton, and D.L. Schacter. 2007. Effects of emotion on memory specificity: Memory trade-offs elicited by negative visually arousing stimuli. Journal of Memory and Language 56: 575–591. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2006.05.004.

Levine, B., E. Svoboda, J.F. Hay, G. Winocur, and M. Moscovitch. 2002. Aging and autobiographical memory: Dissociating episodic from semantic retrieval. Psychology and Aging 17: 677–689. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.17.4.677.

Mahr, J., and G. Csibra. 2018. Why do we remember? The communicative function of episodic memory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 41: e1. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X17000012.

Mar, R.A. 2004. The neuropsychology of narrative: Story comprehension, story production and their interrelation. Neuropsychologia 42: 1414–1434. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.12.016.

McIntosh, A.R. 2007. Coding and representation: The importance of mesoscale dynamics. In Science of memory concepts, ed. H.L. Roediger, Y. Dudai, and S.M. Fitzpatrick, 65–68. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195310443.003.0004.

Menon, V., and L.Q. Uddin. 2010. Saliency, switching, attention and control: A network model of insula function. Brain Structure & Function 214: 655–667. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-010-0262-0.

Michaelian, K. 2011. Generative memory. Philosophical Psychology 24: 323–342. https://doi.org/10.1080/09515089.2011.559623.

Michaelian, K. 2016a. Against discontinuism: Mental time travel and our knowledge of past and future events. In Seeing the future: Theoretical perspectives on future-oriented mental time travel, ed. K. Michaelian, S.B. Klein, and K.K. Szpunar, 62–92. Oxford: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780190241537.003.0004.

Michaelian, K. 2016b. Confabulating, misremembering, relearning: The simulation theory of memory and unsuccessful remembering. Frontiers in Psychology 7: 1857. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01857.

Misra, P., A. Marconi, M. Peterson, and G. Kreiman. 2018. Minimal memory for details in real life events. Scientific Reports 8: 16701. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-33792-2.

Neisser, U. 1976. Cognition and reality: Principles and implications of cognitive psychology. New York, NY: W H Freeman/Times Books/ Henry Holt & Co..

O'Keefe, J., and L. Nadel. 1978. The hippocampus as a cognitive map. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

Okuda, J., T. Fujii, H. Ohtake, T. Tsukiura, K. Tanji, K. Suzuki, R. Kawashima, H. Fukuda, M. Itoh, and A. Yamadori. 2003. Thinking of the future and the past: The roles of the frontal pole and the medial temporal lobes. Neuroimage 19: 1369–1380. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00179-4.

Peer, M., Y. Ron, R. Monsa, and S. Arzy. 2019. Processing of different spatial scales in the human brain. eLife 8: e47492. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.47492.

Perrin, D. 2016. Asymmetries in subjective time. In Seeing the future: Theoretical perspectives on future-oriented mental time travel, ed. K. Michaelian, S.B. Klein, and K.K. Szpunar, 39–61. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780190241537.003.0003.

Perrin, D., and K. Michaelian. 2017. Memory as mental time travel. In The Routledge handbook of philosophy of memory, ed. S. Bernecker and K. Michaelian, 228–240. London: Routledge.

Rasmussen, A.S., and D. Berntsen. 2013. The reality of the past versus the ideality of the future: Emotional valence and functional differences between past and future mental time travel. Memory & Cognition 41: 187–200. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-012-0260-y.

Richardson, A. 2012. Defaulting to fiction: Neuroscience rediscovers the romantic imagination. Poetics Today 32 (4): 663–692. https://doi.org/10.1215/03335372-1459845.

Roberts, R.P., and D.R. Addis. 2018. A common mode of processing governing divergent thinking and future imagination. In The Cambridge handbook of the neuroscience of creativity, ed. R.E. Jung and O. Vartanian, 211–230. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316556238.013.

Roberts, R.P., D.L. Schacter, and D.R. Addis. 2017a. Scene construction and relational processing: Separable constructs? Cerebral Cortex. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhx081.

Roberts, R.P., K. Wiebels, R.L. Sumner, V. van Mulukom, C.L. Grady, D.L. Schacter, and D.R. Addis. 2017b. An fMRI investigation of the relationship between future imagination and cognitive flexibility. Neuropsychologia 95: 156–172. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.11.019.

Robins, S.K. 2016. Misremembering. Philosophical Psychology 29: 432–447. https://doi.org/10.1080/09515089.2015.1113245.

Roediger, H.L., Y. Dudai, and S.M. Fitzpatrick, eds. 2007. Science of memory concepts. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195310443.001.0001.

Rubin, D.C., and S. Umanath. 2015. Event memory: A theory of memory for laboratory, autobiographical, and fictional events. Psychological Review 122: 1–23. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037907.

Rumelhart, D.E., and A. Ortony. 1977. The representation of knowledge in memory. In Schooling and the acquisition of knowledge, ed. R.C. Anderson, R.J. Spiro, and W.E. Montague, 99–135. Hillsdale, N.J: Erlbaum.

Schacter, D.L., and D.R. Addis. 2007. The cognitive neuroscience of constructive memory: Remembering the past and imagining the future. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences 362: 773–786. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2007.2087.

Schacter, D.L., D.R. Addis, and R.L. Buckner. 2007. The prospective brain: Remembering the past to imagine the future. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 8: 657–661. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2213.

Schacter, D.L., K.A. Norman, and W. Koutstaal. 1998. The cognitive neuroscience of constructive memory. Annual Review of Psychology 49: 289–318. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.49.1.289.

Schacter, D.L., and E. Tulving. 1994. What are the memory systems of 1994? In Memory systems, ed. D.L. Schacter and E. Tulving, 1–38. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Schank, R.C., and R.P. Abelson. 1977. Scripts, plans, goals and understanding: An inquiry into human knowledge structures. Oxford: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Scoboria, A., D.L. Jackson, J. Talarico, M. Hanczakowski, L. Wysman, and G. Mazzoni. 2014. The role of belief in occurrence within autobiographical memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 143: 1242–1258. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034110.

Sheldon, S., and B. Levine. 2016. The role of the hippocampus in memory and mental construction. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1369: 76–92. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.13006.

Sheldon, S., M.P. McAndrews, and M. Moscovitch. 2011. Episodic memory processes mediated by the medial temporal lobes contribute to open-ended problem solving. Neuropsychologia 49: 2439–2447. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.04.021.

Shine, J.M., P.G. Bissett, P.T. Bell, O. Koyejo, J.H. Balsters, K.J. Gorgolewski, C.A. Moodie, and R.A. Poldrack. 2016. The dynamics of functional brain networks: Integrated network states during cognitive task performance. Neuron 92: 544–554. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2016.09.018.

Spreng, R.N., W.D. Stevens, J.P. Chamberlain, A.W. Gilmore, and D.L. Schacter. 2010. Default network activity, coupled with the frontoparietal control network, supports goal-directed cognition. Neuroimage 53: 303–317 10.1016%2Fj.neuroimage.2010.06.016.

Squire, L.R., E.T. Rolls, M.K. Johnson, and R.L. Buckner. 2007. Memory systems. In Science of memory: Concepts, ed. H.L. Roediger, Y. Dudai, and S.M. Fitzpatrick, 338–364. New York: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195310443.003.0016.

Squire, L.R., A.S. van der Horst, S.G.R. McDuff, J.C. Frascino, R.O. Hopkinse, and K.N. Mauldin. 2010. Role of the hippocampus in remembering the past and imagining the future. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 107: 19044–19048. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1014391107.

Szpunar, K.K., J.C.K. Chan, and K.B. McDermott. 2009. Contextual processing in episodic future thought. Cerebral Cortex 19: 1539–1548. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhn191.

Szpunar, K.K., J.M. Watson, and K.B. McDermott. 2007. Neural substrates of envisioning the future. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 104: 642–647. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0610082104.

Taylor, S.E., and S.K. Schneider. 1989. Coping and the simulation of events. Social Cognition 7: 174–194. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.1989.7.2.174.

Tolman, E.C. 1948. Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychological Review 55: 189–208. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0061626.

Tulving, E. 1972. Episodic and semantic memory. In Organization of Memory, ed. E. Tulving and W. Donaldson, 381–403. New York: Academic Press.

Tulving, E. 1985. Memory and consciousness. Canadian Psychologist 25: 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0080017.

Urmson, J.O. 1967. Memory and imagination. Mind 76 (301): 83–91.

van Mulukom, V., D.L. Schacter, M.C. Corballis, and D.R. Addis. 2013. Re-imagining the future: Repetition decreases hippocampal involvement in future simulation. PLoS One 8: e69596. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0069596.

Verfaellie, M., A.A. Wank, A.G. Reid, E. Race, and M.M. Keane. 2019. Self-related processing and future thinking: Distinct contributions of ventromedial prefrontal cortex and the medial temporal lobes. Cortex 115: 159–171. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2019.01.028.

Webb, C.E., I.C. Turney, and N.A. Dennis. 2016. What's the gist? The influence of schemas on the neural correlates underlying true and false memories. Neuropsychologia 93: 61–75. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.09.023.

Weiler, J.A., B. Suchan, and I. Daum. 2010. Foreseeing the future: Occurrence probability of imagined future events modulates hippocampal activation. Hippocampus 20: 685–690. https://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.20695.

Wiebels, K., D.R. Addis, D. Moreau, V. van Mulukom, K.E. Onderdijk, and R.P. Roberts. 2020. Relational processing demands and the role of spatial context in the construction of episodic future simulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000831.

Zacks, J.M., N.K. Speer, K.M. Swallow, T.S. Braver, and J.R. Reynolds. 2007. Event perception: A mind/brain perspective. Psychological Bulletin 133: 273–293. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.133.2.273.

Zacks, J.M., and B. Tversky. 2001. Event structure in perception and conception. Psychological Bulletin 127: 3–21. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.127.1.3.