Language input and outcome variation as a test of theory plausibility: The case of early phonological acquisition

Developmental Review - Tập 57 - Trang 100914 - 2020
Alejandrina Cristia1
1Laboratoire de Sciences Cognitives et Psycholinguistique, Dept d’Etudes Cognitives, ENS, PSL University, EHESS, CNRS, 29, Rue d’Ulm, 75005 Paris, France

Tài liệu tham khảo

Ambridge, 2015, A constructivist account of child language acquisition, 478 Anonymous (2020). A systematic review suggests marked differences in the prevalence of infant-directed vocalizations across cultures. Unpublished manuscript. https://xcult.shinyapps.io/vocsr/. (Accessed 08 June 2020). Submitted for publication. Bates, 2001, On the inseparability of grammar and the lexicon: Evidence from acquisition, 134 Bergelson, 2019, Day by day, hour by hour: Naturalistic language input to infants, Developmental Science, 22, e12715, 10.1111/desc.12715 Bergelson, 2019, What do North American babies hear? A large-scale cross-corpus analysis, Developmental Science, 22, e12724, 10.1111/desc.12724 Bergelson, 2012, At 6–9 months, human infants know the meanings of many common nouns, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109, 3253, 10.1073/pnas.1113380109 Bergelson, 2018, Young infants’ word comprehension given an unfamiliar talker or altered pronunciations, Child Development, 89, 1567, 10.1111/cdev.12888 Bergmann, 2018, Promoting replicability in developmental research through meta-analyses: Insights from language acquisition research, Child Development, 89, 1996, 10.1111/cdev.13079 Chambers, 2003, Infants learn phonotactic regularities from brief auditory experience, Cognition, 87, B69, 10.1016/s0010-0277(02)00233-0 Christiansen, 2008, Language as shaped by the brain, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 489, 10.1017/S0140525X08004998 Christiansen, 2016, The now-or-never bottleneck: A fundamental constraint on language, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39, 10.1017/S0140525X1500031X Clark, A., & Lappin, S. (2010). Linguistic nativism and the poverty of the stimulus. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons. Costafreda, 2009, Pooling fMRI data: Meta-analysis, mega-analysis and multi-center studies, Frontiers in Neuroinformatics, 3, 33, 10.3389/neuro.11.033.2009 Cristia, 2014, Neural correlates of infant accent discrimination: An fNIRS study, Developmental Science, 17, 628, 10.1111/desc.12160 Cristia, 2014, Predicting individual variation in language from infant speech perception measures, Child Development, 85, 1330, 10.1111/cdev.12193 Csibra, 2009, Natural pedagogy, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 148, 10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.005 Curtin, 2011, Bilingual beginnings as a lens for theory development: PRIMIR in focus, Journal of Phonetics, 39, 492, 10.1016/j.wocn.2010.12.002 De Lavilléon, 2015, Explicit memory creation during sleep demonstrates a causal role of place cells in navigation, Nature Neuroscience, 18, 493, 10.1038/nn.3970 Dehaene-Lambertz, 2004, Common neural basis for phoneme processing in infants and adults, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 1375, 10.1162/0898929042304714 Dupoux, 2018, Cognitive science in the era of artificial intelligence: A roadmap for reverse-engineering the infant language-learner, Cognition, 173, 43, 10.1016/j.cognition.2017.11.008 Eilers, 1993, The role of prematurity and socioeconomic status in the onset of canonical babbling in infants, Infant Behavior and Development, 16, 297, 10.1016/0163-6383(93)80037-9 Fourtassi, 2014, A rudimentary lexicon and semantics help bootstrap phoneme acquisition, 191 Frank, 2017, A collaborative approach to infant research: Promoting reproducibility, best practices, and theory-building, Infancy, 22, 421, 10.1111/infa.12182 Frank, 2009, Using speakers’ referential intentions to model early cross-situational word learning, Psychological Science, 20, 578, 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02335.x Frank, 2013, The acquisition of anaphora by simple recurrent networks, Language Acquisition, 20, 181, 10.1080/10489223.2013.796950 Frank, 2014, Weak semantic context helps phonetic learning in a model of infant language acquisition, 1073 Goldin-Meadow, 2014, In search of resilient and fragile properties of language, Journal of Child Language, 41, 64, 10.1017/S030500091400021X Goldin-Meadow, 2015, Watching language grow in the manual modality: Nominals, predicates, and handshapes, Cognition, 136, 381, 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.11.029 Goldstein, 2008, Social feedback to infants’ babbling facilitates rapid phonological learning, Psychological Science, 19, 515, 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02117.x Goldwater, 2009, A Bayesian framework for word segmentation: Exploring the effects of context, Cognition, 112, 21, 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.03.008 Golinkoff, 2015, (Baby) talk to me: The social context of infant-directed speech and its effects on early language acquisition, Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24, 339, 10.1177/0963721415595345 Gonzalez-Gomez, 2012, Phonotactic acquisition in healthy preterm infants, Developmental Science, 15, 885, 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01186.x Gros-Louis, 2006, Mothers provide differential feedback to infants’ prelinguistic sounds, International Journal of Behavioral Development, 30, 509, 10.1177/0165025406071914 Hale, 2003, The subset principle in phonology: Why the tabula can’t be rasa, Journal of Linguistics, 39, 219, 10.1017/S0022226703002019 Hart, 1995 Havron, 2019, Three-to four-year-old children rapidly adapt their predictions and use them to learn novel word meanings, Child Development, 90, 82, 10.1111/cdev.13113 Hayes, 2004, Phonological acquisition in optimality theory: The early stages, 158 Heinz, 2016, Computational theories of learning and developmental psycholinguistics, 633 Horlyck, 2012, The relationship between learning to read and language-specific speech perception: Maturation versus experience, Scientific Studies of Reading, 16, 218, 10.1080/10888438.2010.546460 Imai, 2015, Sound symbolism facilitates word learning in 14-month-olds, PLoS One, 10, e0116494, 10.1371/journal.pone.0116494 Johnson, 2003, Detecting agents, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 358, 549, 10.1098/rstb.2002.1237 Johnson, 2008, Unsupervised word segmentation for Sesotho using adaptor grammars, 20 Johnson, M., Christophe, A., Dupoux, E., & Demuth, K. (2014). Modelling function words improves unsupervised word segmentation. In ACL (pp. 282–292). Jusczyk, 1993, From general to language-specific capacities: The WRAPSA model of how speech perception develops, Journal of Phonetics, 21, 3, 10.1016/S0095-4470(19)31319-1 Jusczyk, 1994, Infants’ sensitivity to phonotactic patterns in the native language, Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 630, 10.1006/jmla.1994.1030 Kaplan, 2008, Computational models in the debate over language learnability, Infant and Child Development: An International Journal of Research and Practice, 17, 55, 10.1002/icd.544 Kuhl, 1991, Human adults and human infants show a perceptual magnet effect for the prototypes of speech categories, monkeys do not, Perception and Psychophysics, 50, 93, 10.3758/BF03212211 Kuhl, 2007, Is speech learning ’gated’ by the social brain?, Developmental Science, 10, 110, 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00572.x Kuhl, 2003, Foreign-language experience in infancy: Effects of short-term exposure and social interaction on phonetic learning, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100, 9096, 10.1073/pnas.1532872100 Lakens, 2020, Improving inferences about null effects with Bayes factors and equivalence tests, The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, 75, 45, 10.1093/geronb/gby065 Lenneberg, 1969, On explaining language, Science, 164, 635, 10.1126/science.164.3880.635 Lieven, 2010, Language development in a cross-linguistic context, 91 MacWhinney, 2000 Magri, 2013, The complexity of learning in optimality theory and its implications for the acquisition of phonotactics, Linguistic Inquiry, 44, 434, 10.1162/LING_a_00134 Magri, 2014, Error-driven versus batch models of the acquisition of phonotactics: David defeats Goliath, Proceedings of the Annual Meetings on Phonology, 10.3765/amp.v1i1.51 Majorano, 2014, The relationship between infants’ production experience and their processing of speech, Language Learning and Development, 10, 179, 10.1080/15475441.2013.829740 Maye, 2002, Infant sensitivity to distributional information can effect phonetic discrimination, Cognition, 82, B101, 10.1016/S0010-0277(01)00157-3 McCauley, 2019, Language learning as language use: A cross-linguistic model of child language development, Psychological Review, 126, 1, 10.1037/rev0000126 McGillion, 2017, What paves the way to conventional language? The predictive value of babble, pointing, and socioeconomic status, Child Development, 88, 156, 10.1111/cdev.12671 Melvin, 2017, Home environment, but not socioeconomic status, is linked to differences in early phonetic perception ability, Infancy, 22, 42, 10.1111/infa.12145 Mollica, 2019, Humans store about 1.5 megabytes of information during language acquisition, Royal Society Open Science, 6, 181393, 10.1098/rsos.181393 Moon, 1993, Two-day-olds prefer their native language, Infant Behavior and Development, 16, 495, 10.1016/0163-6383(93)80007-U Nespor, 2008, Different phrasal prominence realizations in VO and OV languages, Lingue e Linguaggio, 7, 139 Ochs, 2001, Language acquisition and socialization: Three developmental stories and their implications, 263 Oller, 1995, Extreme poverty and the development of precursors to the speech capacity, First Language, 15, 167, 10.1177/014272379501504403 Oller, 1994, Speech-like vocalizations in infancy: An evaluation of potential risk factors, Journal of Child Language, 21, 33, 10.1017/S0305000900008667 Pace, 2017, Identifying pathways between socioeconomic status and language development, Annual Review of Linguistics, 3, 285, 10.1146/annurev-linguistics-011516-034226 Pearl, 2019 Pearl, 2013, Parameters in language acquisition, 129 Petitto, 2004, Baby hands that move to the rhythm of language: Hearing babies acquiring sign languages babble silently on the hands, Cognition, 93, 43, 10.1016/j.cognition.2003.10.007 Phillips, 2015, The utility of cognitive plausibility in language acquisition modeling: Evidence from word segmentation, Cognitive Science, 39, 1824, 10.1111/cogs.12217 Pierrehumbert, 2003, Phonetic diversity, statistical learning, and acquisition of phonology, Language and Speech, 46, 115, 10.1177/00238309030460020501 Pierrehumbert, 2016, Phonological representation: Beyond abstract versus episodic, Annual Review of Linguistics, 2, 33, 10.1146/annurev-linguistics-030514-125050 Poeppel, 2014, The neuroanatomic and neurophysiological infrastructure for speech and language, Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 28, 142, 10.1016/j.conb.2014.07.005 Ramírez-Esparza, 2014, Look who’s talking: Speech style and social context in language input to infants are linked to concurrent and future speech development, Developmental Science, 17, 880, 10.1111/desc.12172 Räsänen, 2012, Computational modeling of phonetic and lexical learning in early language acquisition: Existing models and future directions, Speech Communication, 54, 975, 10.1016/j.specom.2012.05.001 Rogoff, 2003, Firsthand learning through intent participation, Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 175, 10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145118 Rose, 2014, The PhonBank Project: Data and software-assisted methods for the study of phonology and phonological development, 633 Sandler, 2005, The emergence of grammar: Systematic structure in a new language, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102, 2661, 10.1073/pnas.0405448102 Shneidman, 2013, What counts as effective input for word learning?, Journal of Child Language, 40, 672, 10.1017/S0305000912000141 Shneidman, 2016, Are child-directed interactions the cradle of social learning?, Psychological Bulletin, 142, 1, 10.1037/bul0000023 Scaff, C., & Cristia, A. (2019, manuscript in preparation). Socioeconomic status and early lexical processing: A meta-analysis. Shneidman, L. A. (2010). Language input and acquisition in a Mayan village (PhD thesis). The University of Chicago. Smith, 2008, Infants rapidly learn word-referent mappings via cross-situational statistics, Cognition, 106, 1558, 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.06.010 Smolensky, 2006 Swingley, 2009, Contributions of infant word learning to language development, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364, 3617, 10.1098/rstb.2009.0107 Trueswell, 2013, Propose but verify: Fast mapping meets cross-situational word learning, Cognitive Psychology, 66, 126, 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2012.10.001 Tsuji, 2014, Perceptual attunement in vowels: A meta-analysis, Developmental Psychobiology, 56, 179, 10.1002/dev.21179 Tsuji, S., Dupoux, E., & Cristia, A. (2019, manuscript in preparation). A framework for social theories of language acquisition. Versteegh, 2015, The zero resource speech challenge Vouloumanos, 2014, Listen up! Speech is for thinking during infancy, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 642, 10.1016/j.tics.2014.10.001 Warlaumont, 2014, A social feedback loop for speech development and its reduction in autism, Psychological Science, 25, 1314, 10.1177/0956797614531023 Weber, 2017, When cultural norms discourage talking to babies: Effectiveness of a parenting program in rural Senegal, Child Development, 88, 1513, 10.1111/cdev.12882 Weisleder, 2013, Talking to children matters: Early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary, Psychological Science, 24, 2143, 10.1177/0956797613488145 Werker, 2005, PRIMIR: A developmental framework of infant speech processing, Language Learning and Development, 1, 197, 10.1080/15475441.2005.9684216 Werker, 2015, Critical periods in speech perception: New directions, Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 173, 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015104 Werker, 1984, Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life, Infant Behavior and Development, 7, 49, 10.1016/S0163-6383(84)80022-3 Yang, 2002 Yurovsky, 2018, A communicative approach to early word learning, New Ideas in Psychology, 50, 73, 10.1016/j.newideapsych.2017.09.001