Is Phonological Encoding in Naming Influenced by Literacy?

Paulo Ventura1, Régine Kolinsky2, Luís Querido1, Sandra Fernandes1, José Morais2
1Faculdade de Psicologia e de Ciências da Educação, Universidade de Lisboa, 1649-013 Lisboa, Portugal
2Research Unit in Cognitive Neurosciences (UNeSCog), Université Libre de Bruxelles, Bruxelles, Belgium

Tóm tắt

Từ khóa


Tài liệu tham khảo

Belke E. (2005). Lexical access revisited: Evidence from picture-pseudoword interference. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 17, 117–150

Bonin P., Fayol M., Gombert J.-E. (1997). Role of phonological and orthographic codes in picture naming and writing: An interference paradigm study. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive/Current Psychology of Cognition 16, 299–324

Bonin P., Peereman R., Malardier N., Méot A., Chalard M. (2003). A new set of 299 pictures for psycholinguistic studies: French norms for name agreement, image agreement, conceptual familiarity, visual complexity, image variability, age of acquisition, and naming latencies. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers 35, 158–167

Brooks P.J., MacWhinney B. (2000). Phonological priming in children’s picture naming. Journal of Child Language 27, 335–366

Charles-Luce J., Luce P.A. (1995). An examination of similarity neighbourhoods in young children’s receptive vocabularies. Journal of Child Language 22, 727–735

Coady J.A., Aslin R.N. (2003). Phonological neighbourhoods in the developing lexicon. Journal of Child Language 30, 441–469

Coady J.A., Aslin R.N. (2004). Young children’s sensitivity to probabilistic phonotactics in the developing lexicon. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 89, 183–213

Cutting J.C., Ferreira V.S. (1999). Semantic and phonological information flow in the production lexicon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 25, 318–344

Cycowicz Y.M., Friedman D., Rothstein M., Snodgrass J.G. (1997). Picture naming by young children: Norms for name agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 65, 171–237

Damian M., Martin R.C. (1999). Semantic and phonological codes interact in single word production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 25: 345–361

Damian M., Bowers J.S. (2003). Effects of orthography on speech production in a form-preparation paradigm. Journal of Memory and Language 49, 119–132

de Gelder B., Vroomen J., Bertelson P. (1993). The effects of alphabetic-reading competence on language representation in bilingual Chinese subjects. Psychological Research 55, 315–321

Fromkin V.A. (1971). The non-anomalous nature of anomalous utterances. Language 47, 27–52

Garlock V.M., Walley A.C., Metsala J.L. (2001). Age-of-acquisition, word frequency, neighborhood density effects on spoken word recognition by children and adults. Journal of Memory and Language 45, 468–492

Glaser W.R., Düngelhoff F.-J. (1984). The time course of picture-word interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 10, 640–654

Goswami U. (2000). Phonological representations, reading development and dyslexia: Towards a cross-linguistic theoretical framework. Dyslexia 6, 133–151

Goswami U. (2002a). Phonology, reading development and dyslexia: A cross-linguistic perspective. Annals of Dyslexia 52, 141–163

Goswami U. (2002b). Phonology, learning to read and dyslexia: A cross-linguistic analysis. In: Csepe V.(eds) Dyslexia: Different brain, different behaviour. NL, Kluwer Academic, pp. 1–40

Humphreys G.W., Riddoch M.J., Quinlan P.T. (1988). Cascade processes in picture identification. Cognitive Neuropsychology 5, 67–103

Jerger S., Martin R.C., Damian M.F. (2002). Semantic and phonological influences on picture naming by children and teenagers. Journal of Memory and Language 47, 229–249

Jescheniak J.D., Schriefers H. (2001). Priming effects from phonologically related distractors in picture-word interference. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 54A: 371–382

Levelt W.J.M., Roelofs A., Meyer A.S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22, 1–75

Lupker S.J. (1982). The role of phonetic and orthographic similarity in picture-word interference. Canadian Journal of Psychology 36, 349–367

Marslen-Wilson W.D. (1987). Functional parallelism in spoken word recognition. Cognition 25, 71–102

McClelland J.L., Elman J.L. (1986). The TRACE model of speech perception. Cognitive Psychology 18, 1–86

Metsala J.L. (1997). An examination of word frequency and neighborhood density in the development of spoken-word recognition. Memory and Cognition 25, 47–56

Metsala, J. L., & Walley, A. C. (1998). Spoken vocabulary growth and the segmental restructuring of lexical representations: Precursors to phonemic awareness and early reading ability. In J. L. Metsala & L. C. Ehri (Eds.), Word recognition in beginning literacy (pp. 89–120). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Meyer A.S. (1990). The time course of phonological encoding in language production: The encoding of successive syllables of a word. Journal of Memory and Language 29, 524–545

Meyer A.S. (1991). The time course of phonological encoding in language production: The phonological encoding inside a syllable. Journal of Memory and Language 30, 69–89

Meyer A.S., Schriefers H. (1991). Phonological facilitation in picture-word interference experiments: Effects of stimulus onset asynchrony and types of interfering stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 17: 1146–1160

Morais J., Bertelson P., Cary L., Alegria J. (1986). Literacy training and speech analysis. Cognition 24, 45–64

Morais J., Cary L., Alegria J., Bertelson P. (1979). Does awareness of speech as a sequence of phones arise spontaneously?. Cognition 7, 323–331

Morais, J., & Kolinsky, R. (2001). The literate mind and the universal human mind. In E. Dupoux (Ed.), Language, brain and cognitive development: Essays in honor of Jacques Mehler (pp. 463–480). Cambridge, MA: MIT Also in E. Dupoux (Ed.), Les Langages du cerveau: autour de Jacques Mehler, Ed. Odile Jacob, 2002.

Morais J., Kolinsky R. (2002). Literacy effects on language and cognition. In: Bäckman L., von Hofsten C.(eds) Pyschology at the turn of the millennium. Hove, UK, Psychology Press, pp. 507–530

Morais J., Kolinsky R. (2005). Literacy and cognitive change. In: Snowling M., Hulme Ch.(eds) The science of reading. A handbook. Oxford, Blackwell, pp. 188–203

Morsella E., Miozzo M. (2002). Evidence for a cascade model of lexical access in speech production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 28: 555–563

Muneaux M., Ziegler J.C. (2004). Locus of orthographic effects in spoken word recognition: Novel insights from the neighbour generation task. Language and Cognitive Processes 19: 641–660

Navarrete E., Costa A. (2005). Phonological activation of ignored pictures: Further evidence for a cascade model of lexical access. Journal of Memory and Language 53, 359–377

Norris D.G. (1994). Shortlist: A conectionist model of continuous speech recognition. Cognition 52, 189–234

Peterson K.M., Reis A., Ingvar M. (2001). Cognitive processing in literate and illiterate subjects: A review of some recent behavioral and functional neuroimaging data. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 42, 251–267

Peterson R.R., Savoy P. (1998). Lexical selection and phonological encoding during language production: Evidence for cascaded processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 24: 539–557

Posnansky C.J., Rayner K. (1977). Visual-feature and response components in a picture-word interference task with beginning and skilled readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 24, 440–449

Rayner K., Springer C.J. (1986). Graphemic and semantic similarity effects in the picture-word interference task. British Journal of Psychology 77, 207–222

Read C., Zhang Y., Nie H., Ding B. (1986). The ability to manipulate speech sounds depends on knowing alphabetic reading. Cognition 24, 31–44

Roelofs, A., Meyer, A. S., & Levelt, W. J. M. (1996). Interaction between semantic and orthographic factors in conceptually driven naming: Comment on Starreveld and La Heij (1995). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 246–251.

Schneider W., Eschman A., Zuccolotto A. (2002a). E-Prime reference guide. Pittsburgh, Psychology Software Tools

Schneider W., Eschman A., Zuccolotto A. (2002b). E-Prime user’s guide. Pittsburgh, Psychology Software Tools

Schriefers H., Meyer A.S., Levelt W.J.M. (1990). Exploring the time course of lexical access in language production: Picture-word interference studies. Journal of Memory and Language 29, 86–102

Seidenberg M.S., Tanenhaus M.K. (1979). Orthographic effects on rhyme monitoring. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory 5: 546–554

Snodgrass J.G., Vanderwart M. (1980). A standardized set of 260 pictures: norms for name agreement, image agreement, and visual omplexity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory 6:174–215

Starreveld P.A. (2000). On the interpretation of onsets of auditory context effects in word production. Journal of Memory and Language 42, 497–525

Starreveld P.A., La Heij W. (1995). Semantic interference, orthographic facilitation and their interaction in naming tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 21: 686–698

Starreveld P.A., La Heij W. (1996). Time-course analysis of semantic and orthographic context effects in picture naming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 22: 896–918

Starreveld P.A., La Heij W. (2004). Phonological facilitation of grammatical gender retrieval. Language and Cognitive Processes 19, 677–711

Storkel H.L. (2002). Restructuring of similarity neighborhoods in the developing mental lexicon. Journal of Child Language 29, 251–274

Treiman R. (1983). The structure of spoken syllables: Evidence from novel word games. Cognition 15, 49–74

Treiman R. (1986). The division between onsets and rimes in English syllables. Journal of Memory and Language 25, 476–491

Ventura P., Kolinsky R., Brito-Mendes C., Morais J. (2001). Mental representations of the syllable internal structure are influenced by orthography. Language and Cognitive Processes 16, 393–418

Ventura, P., Kolinsky, R., Fernandes, S., Querido, J.-L., & Morais, J. (in press). Lexical restructuring in the absence of literacy. Cognition.

Ventura P., Morais J., Pattamadilok C., Kolinsky R. (2004). The locus of the orthographic consistency effect in auditory word recognition. Language and Cognitive Processes 19, 57–95

Walley A.C., Metsala J.L., Garlock V.M. (2003). Spoken vocabulary growth: Its role in the development of phoneme awareness and early reading ability. Reading and Writing 16, 5–20

Wheeldon L.R., Monsell S. (1992). The locus of repetition priming of spoken word production. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 44A: 723–761

Ziegler J.C., Ferrand L. (1998). Orthography shapes the perception of speech: The consistency effect in auditory recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 5: 683–689