Information integration without awareness
Tài liệu tham khảo
1964
Kant, 1998
James, 1890
Suchow, 2013, Learning to detect and combine the features of an object, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A., 110, 785, 10.1073/pnas.1218438110
Treisman, 2003, Consciousness and perceptual binding, 95
Griffith, V. and Koch, C. (2012) Quantifying synergistic mutual information. arXiv 1205.4265
Balduzzi, 2008, Integrated information in discrete dynamical systems: motivation and theoretical framework, PLoS Comput. Biol., 4, e1000091, 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000091
Engel, 1999, Temporal binding, binocular rivalry, and consciousness, Conscious. Cogn., 8, 128, 10.1006/ccog.1999.0389
Baars, 2002, The conscious access hypothesis: origins and recent evidence, Trends Cogn. Sci., 6, 47, 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01819-2
Damasio, 1999
Fahrenfort, 2012, A true science of consciousness explains phenomenology: comment on Cohen and Dennett, Trends Cogn. Sci., 16, 138, 10.1016/j.tics.2012.01.004
Dehaene, 2001, Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: basic evidence and a workspace framework, Cognition, 79, 1, 10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00123-2
Edelman, 2000
Tononi, 2013, Integrated information theory of consciousness: an updated account, Arch. Ital. Biol., 150, 290
Kanwisher, 2001, Neural events and perceptual awareness, Cognition, 79, 89, 10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00125-6
Koch, 2012
Marcel, 1983, Conscious and unconscious perception: an approach to the relations between phenomenal experience and perceptual processes, Cogn. Psychol., 15, 238, 10.1016/0010-0285(83)90010-5
Varela, 2001, The brainweb: phase synchronization and large-scale integration, Nat. Rev. Neurosci., 2, 229, 10.1038/35067550
Crick, 2003, A framework for consciousness, Nat. Neurosci., 6, 119, 10.1038/nn0203-119
Cheesman, 1984, Priming with and without awareness, Percept. Psychophys., 36, 387, 10.3758/BF03202793
Baars, 2013, Global workspace dynamics: cortical ‘binding and propagation’ enables conscious contents, Front. Psychol., 4, 200, 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00200
Searle, 1992
Lamme, 2006, Towards a true neural stance on consciousness, Trends Cogn. Sci., 10, 494, 10.1016/j.tics.2006.09.001
Lamme, 2000, The distinct modes of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent processing, Trends Neurosci., 23, 571, 10.1016/S0166-2236(00)01657-X
Dehaene, 2011, Experimental and theoretical approaches to conscious processing, Neuron, 70, 200, 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.03.018
Dehaene, 2014, Toward a computational theory of conscious processing, Curr. Opin. Neurobiol., 25, 76, 10.1016/j.conb.2013.12.005
Hubel, 1962, Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex, J. Physiol., 160, 106, 10.1113/jphysiol.1962.sp006837
Kouider, 2007, Levels of processing during non-conscious perception: a critical review of visual masking, Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci., 362, 857, 10.1098/rstb.2007.2093
Lin, 2009, Seeing the invisible: the scope and limits of unconscious processing in binocular rivalry, Prog. Neurobiol., 87, 195, 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2008.09.002
Kim, 2005, Psychophysical magic: rendering the visible ‘invisible’, Trends Cogn. Sci., 9, 381, 10.1016/j.tics.2005.06.012
Tsuchiya, 2005, Continuous flash suppression reduces negative afterimages, Nat. Neurosci., 8, 1096, 10.1038/nn1500
Schmidt, 2000, Visual perception without awareness: priming responses by color, 157
Blake, 1974, Adaptation to invisible gratings and the site of binocular rivalry suppression, Nature, 249, 488, 10.1038/249488a0
Treisman, 1980, A feature-integration theory of attention, Cogn. Psychol., 12, 97, 10.1016/0010-0285(80)90005-5
Vorberg, 2003, Different time courses for visual perception and action priming, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A., 100, 6275, 10.1073/pnas.0931489100
Dehaene, 1998, Imaging unconscious semantic priming, Nature, 395, 597, 10.1038/26967
Dehaene, 2001, Cerebral mechanisms of word masking and unconscious repetition priming, Nat. Neurosci., 4, 752, 10.1038/89551
Kouider, 2009, Activity in face-responsive brain regions is modulated by invisible, attended faces: evidence from masked priming, Cereb. Cortex, 19, 13, 10.1093/cercor/bhn048
Whalen, 2004, Human amygdala responsivity to masked fearful eye whites, Science, 306, 2061, 10.1126/science.1103617
Mudrik, 2011, Integration without awareness: expanding the limits of unconscious processing, Psychol. Sci., 22, 764, 10.1177/0956797611408736
Mudrik, 2013, Differential processing of invisible congruent and incongruent scenes: a case for unconscious integration, J. Vis., 13, 24, 10.1167/13.13.24
Faivre, 2014, Inferring the direction of implied motion depends on visual awareness, J. Vis., 14, 4, 10.1167/14.4.4
Rajimehr, 2004, Adaptation to apparent motion in crowding condition, Vision Res., 44, 925, 10.1016/j.visres.2003.11.020
Kaunitz, 2011, Unseen complex motion is modulated by attention and generates a visible aftereffect, J. Vis., 11, 10, 10.1167/11.13.10
Faivre, 2012, Nonconscious emotional processing involve distinct neural pathways for pictures and videos, Neuropsychologia, 50, 3736, 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.10.025
Ric, 2012, Unconscious addition: when we unconsciously initiate and follow arithmetic rules, J. Exp. Psychol. Gen., 141, 222, 10.1037/a0024608
VanRullen, 2003, Is perception discrete or continuous?, Trends Cogn. Sci., 7, 207, 10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00095-0
Blake, 2005, The role of temporal structure in human vision, Behav. Cogn. Neurosci. Rev., 4, 21, 10.1177/1534582305276839
Atas, 2014, Nonconscious learning from crowded sequences, Psychol. Sci., 25, 113, 10.1177/0956797613499591
Reber, 2012, Integrating unseen events over time, Conscious. Cogn., 21, 953, 10.1016/j.concog.2012.02.013
Sackur, 2009, The cognitive architecture for chaining of two mental operations, Cognition, 111, 187, 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.01.010
Faivre, 2014, Temporal structure coding with and without awareness, Cognition, 131, 404, 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.02.008
Fisch, 2009, Neural ‘ignition’: enhanced activation linked to perceptual awareness in human ventral stream visual cortex, Neuron, 64, 562, 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.11.001
Van Opstal, 2011, Setting the stage subliminally: unconscious context effects, Conscious. Cogn., 20, 1860, 10.1016/j.concog.2011.09.004
Van Opstal, 2010, Unconscious task application, Conscious. Cogn., 19, 999, 10.1016/j.concog.2010.05.002
Lin, 2014, Unconscious processing of an abstract concept, Psychol. Sci., 25, 296, 10.1177/0956797613504964
Armstrong, 2013, Subliminal understanding of negation: unconscious control by subliminal processing of word pairs, Conscious. Cogn., 22, 1022, 10.1016/j.concog.2013.06.010
Van Opstal, 2011, Rapid parallel semantic processing of numbers without awareness, Cognition, 120, 136, 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.03.005
Sklar, 2012, Reading and doing arithmetic nonconsciously, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A., 109, 19614, 10.1073/pnas.1211645109
García-Orza, 2009, ‘2 x 3’ primes naming ‘6’: evidence from masked priming, Atten. Percept. Psychophys., 71, 471, 10.3758/APP.71.3.471
Driver, 2008, Multisensory interplay reveals crossmodal influences on ‘sensory-specific’brain regions, neural responses, and judgments, Neuron, 57, 11, 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.12.013
Chen, 2010, When hearing the bark helps to identify the dog: semantically-congruent sounds modulate the identification of masked pictures, Cognition, 114, 389, 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.10.012
Chen, 2011, Crossmodal constraints on human perceptual awareness: auditory semantic modulation of binocular rivalry, Front. Psychol., 2, 212, 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00212
Palmer, 2012, The function of consciousness in multisensory integration, Cognition, 125, 353, 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.08.003
Alsius, 2013, Detection of audiovisual speech correspondences without visual awareness, Psychol. Sci., 24, 423, 10.1177/0956797612457378
Ngo, 2010, Crossmodal facilitation of masked visual target identification, Atten. Percept. Psychophys., 72, 1938, 10.3758/APP.72.7.1938
Doi, 2013, Unconscious presentation of fearful face modulates electrophysiological responses to emotional prosody, Cereb. Cortex
Lupyan, 2013, Language can boost otherwise unseen objects into visual awareness, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A., 110, 14196, 10.1073/pnas.1303312110
Lunghi, 2010, Touch disambiguates rivalrous perception at early stages of visual analysis, Curr. Biol., 20, R143, 10.1016/j.cub.2009.12.015
Salomon, 2013, Posing for awareness: proprioception modulates access to visual consciousness in a continuous flash suppression task, J. Vis., 13, 1, 10.1167/13.7.2
Zhou, 2010, Olfaction modulates visual perception in binocular rivalry, Curr. Biol., 20, 1356, 10.1016/j.cub.2010.05.059
Arzi, 2012, Humans can learn new information during sleep, Nat. Neurosci., 15, 1460, 10.1038/nn.3193
Chen, 2011, The crossmodal facilitation of visual object representations by sound: evidence from the backward masking paradigm, J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform., 37, 1784, 10.1037/a0025638
McGurk, 1976, Hearing lips and seeing voices, Nature, 264, 746, 10.1038/264746a0
Mudrik, 2010, ERP evidence for context congruity effects during simultaneous object-scene processing, Neuropsychologia, 48, 507, 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.10.011
Biederman, 1982, Scene perception: detecting and judging objects undergoing relational violations, Cogn. Psychol., 14, 143, 10.1016/0010-0285(82)90007-X
Bar, 2004, Visual objects in context, Nat. Rev. Neurosci., 5, 617, 10.1038/nrn1476
Mudrik, 2011, Scene congruency biases binocular rivalry, Conscious. Cogn., 20, 756, 10.1016/j.concog.2011.01.001
Underwood, 2008, Is attention necessary for object identification? Evidence from eye movements during the inspection of real-world scenes, Conscious. Cogn., 17, 159, 10.1016/j.concog.2006.11.008
Kiesel, 2009, Playing chess unconsciously, J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn., 35, 292, 10.1037/a0014499
Pessiglione, 2008, Subliminal instrumental conditioning demonstrated in the human brain, Neuron, 59, 561, 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.07.005
Raio, 2012, Nonconscious fear is quickly acquired but swiftly forgotten, Curr. Biol., 22, R477, 10.1016/j.cub.2012.04.023
Seitz, 2009, Rewards evoke learning of unconsciously processed visual stimuli in adult humans, Neuron, 61, 700, 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.01.016
Knight, 2003, Expression of conditional fear with and without awareness, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A., 100, 15280, 10.1073/pnas.2535780100
Reber, 2012, Unconscious relational inference recruits the hippocampus, J. Neurosci., 32, 6138, 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5639-11.2012
Blake, 2006, Strength of early visual adaptation depends on visual awareness, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A., 103, 4783, 10.1073/pnas.0509634103
Lau, 2009, Volition and the functions of consciousness, 1191
Crick, 1990, Towards a neurobiological theory of consciousness, Semin. Neurosci., 2, 273
Goodale, 2004, Perceiving the world and grasping it: dissociations between conscious and unconscious visual processing, 1159
Singer, 1998, Consciousness and the structure of neuronal representations, Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci., 353, 1829, 10.1098/rstb.1998.0335
Tononi, 1998, Consciousness and the integration of information in the brain, 245
Nissen, 1987, Attentional requirements of learning: evidence from performance measures, Cogn. Psychol., 19, 1, 10.1016/0010-0285(87)90002-8
Tononi, 2008, Consciousness as integrated information: a provisional manifesto, Biol. Bull., 215, 216, 10.2307/25470707
Tononi, 2010, Information integration: its relevance to brain function and consciousness, Arch. Ital. Biol., 148, 299
Ariely, 2001, Seeing sets: representation by statistical properties, Psychol. Sci., 12, 157, 10.1111/1467-9280.00327
Alvarez, 2011, Representing multiple objects as an ensemble enhances visual cognition, Trends Cogn. Sci., 15, 122, 10.1016/j.tics.2011.01.003
Oriet, 2012, Size averaging of irrelevant stimuli cannot be prevented, Vision Res., 79, 8, 10.1016/j.visres.2012.12.004
Choo, 2010, Objects with reduced visibility still contribute to size averaging, Atten. Percept. Psychophys., 72, 86, 10.3758/APP.72.1.86
Corbett, 2012, An aftereffect of adaptation to mean size, Vis. Cogn., 20, 211, 10.1080/13506285.2012.657261
Dehaene, 2014, Toward a computational theory of conscious processing, Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 25, 76, 10.1016/j.conb.2013.12.005
Frässle, 2014, Binocular Rivalry: Frontal Activity Relates to Introspection and Action But Not to Perception, The Journal of Neuroscience, 34, 1738, 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4403-13.2014
Deroy, 2014, Multisensory constraints on awareness, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369, 20130207, 10.1098/rstb.2013.0207
Oizumi, 2014, From the phenomenology to the mechanisms of consciousness: integrated information theory 3.0, PLoS Comput. Biol., 10, e1003588, 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003588