Infant's perception of goal-directed actions performed by a mechanical device

Infant Behavior and Development - Tập 28 - Trang 466-480 - 2005
Tanja Hofer1, Petra Hauf1, Gisa Aschersleben1
1Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Amalienstrasse 33, 80799 Munich, Germany

Tài liệu tham khảo

Baron-Cohen, 1995 Bayley, 1993 Boudreau, 2000, Spilling thoughts: Configuring attentional resources in infants’ goal-directed actions, Infant Behavior & Development, 23, 543, 10.1016/S0163-6383(01)00059-5 Brown, 1990, Domain-specific principles affect learning and transfer in children, Cognitive Science, 14, 107, 10.1207/s15516709cog1401_6 Carey, 1994, Domain-specific knowledge and conceptual change, 169 Connolly, 1989, The emergence of tool-using skill in infancy, Developmental Psychology, 25, 894, 10.1037/0012-1649.25.6.894 Csibra, 2003, One-year-old infants use teleological representations of actions productively, Cognitive Science, 27, 111, 10.1207/s15516709cog2701_4 Csibra, 1999, Goal attribution without agency cues: The perception of “pure reason” in infancy, Cognition, 72, 237, 10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00039-6 Geisser, 1958, An extension of Box's results on the use of F-distribution in multivariate analysis, Annals of Mathematical Statistics, 29, 885, 10.1214/aoms/1177706545 Gergely, 1995, Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age, Cognition, 56, 165, 10.1016/0010-0277(95)00661-H Johnson, 2000, The recognition of mentalistic agents in infancy, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 22, 10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01414-X Johnson, 2003, Detecting agents, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences, 358, 549, 10.1098/rstb.2002.1237 Johnson, 2001, Inferring the goals of a nonhuman agent, Cognitive Development, 16, 637, 10.1016/S0885-2014(01)00043-0 Johnson, 1998, Whose gaze will infants follow? The elicitation of gaze-following in 12-month-olds, Developmental Science, 1, 233, 10.1111/1467-7687.00036 Jovanovic, B., Király, I., Elsner, B., Gergely, G., Prinz, W., & Aschersleben, G. (submitted for publication). The role of effects for infant's perception of action goals. Király, 2003, The early origins of goal attribution in infancy, Consciousness & Cognition, 12, 732, 10.1016/S1053-8100(03)00084-9 Kuhlmeier, 2003, Attribution of dispositional states by 12-month-olds, Psychological Science, 14, 402, 10.1111/1467-9280.01454 Leslie, 1994, ToMM, ToBy, and Agency: Core architecture and domain specificity, 119 Leslie, 1995, A theory of agency, 121 Lockman, 2000, A perception-action perspective on tool use development, Child Development, 71, 137, 10.1111/1467-8624.00127 Luo, Y., & Baillargeon, R. (in press). Can a self-propelled box have a goal? Psychological reasoning in 5-month-old infants. Psychological Science. McCarty, 2000, Actions with tools, Enfance, 52, 249, 10.3406/enfan.2000.3181 McCarty, 2001, The beginnings of tool use by infants and toddlers, Infancy, 2, 233, 10.1207/S15327078IN0202_8 Meltzoff, 1995, Understanding the intentions of others: Re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children, Developmental Psychology, 31, 838, 10.1037/0012-1649.31.5.838 Modena, 1998, Capacità imitative per l’acquisizione di un compito di uso di strumenti: Un’indagine in bambini nel secondo anno di vita, Età evolutiva, 59, 11 Munakata, 1997, Rethinking infant knowledge: Toward an adaptive process account of successes and failures in object permanence tasks, Psychological Review, 104, 686, 10.1037/0033-295X.104.4.686 Phillips, 2002, Infants’ ability to connect gaze and emotional expression to intentional action, Cognition, 85, 53, 10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00073-2 Piaget, 1963 Pinto, J. (1995). MacXhab 1.4. Stanford, CA. Poulin-Dubois, 1996, Infants’ concept of animacy, Cognitive Development, 11, 19, 10.1016/S0885-2014(96)90026-X Premack, 1990, The infant's theory of self-propelled objects., Cognition, 36, 1, 10.1016/0010-0277(90)90051-K Premack, 1997, Infants attribute value+- to the goal-directed actions of self-propelled objects, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 848, 10.1162/jocn.1997.9.6.848 Provasi, 2001, Do 9-and 12-month-olds learn means-ends relation by observing?, Infant Behavior & Development, 24, 195, 10.1016/S0163-6383(01)00072-8 Schlesinger, 1999, Infants’ developing expectations of possible and impossible tool-use events between ages 8 and 12 months, Developmental Science, 2, 195, 10.1111/1467-7687.00068 Shimizu, 2004, Infants’ attribution of a goal to a morphologically unfamiliar agent, Developmental Science, 7, 425, 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00362.x Sodian, 2004, Do infants apply the principle of rational action to human agents?, Infant Behavior & Development, 27, 31, 10.1016/j.infbeh.2003.05.006 Sodian, 2004, Infants’ understanding of looking, pointing and reaching as cues to goal-directed action, Journal of Cognition and Development, 53, 289, 10.1207/s15327647jcd0503_1 Sommerville, J. A., & Woodward, A. L. (2005). Pulling out the intentional structure of action: The relation between action processing and action production in infancy. Cognition, 95(1), 1–30. Tomasello, 1999, Having intentions, understanding intentions, and understanding communicative intentions, 63 Willatts, 1999, Development of means-end behavior in young infants: Pulling a support to retrieve a distant object, Developmental Psychology, 35, 651, 10.1037/0012-1649.35.3.651 Woodward, 1998, Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach., Cognition, 69, 1, 10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00058-4 Woodward, 1999, Infant's ability to distinguish between purposeful and non-purposeful behaviors, Infant Behavior & Development, 22, 145, 10.1016/S0163-6383(99)00007-7 Woodward, 2003, Infants’ developing understanding of the link between looker and object, Developmental Science, 6, 297, 10.1111/1467-7687.00286 Woodward, 2002, Infants’ understanding of the point gesture as an object-directed action, Cognitive Development, 17, 1061, 10.1016/S0885-2014(02)00074-6 Woodward, 2000, Twelve-month-old infants interpret action in context, Psychological Science, 11, 73, 10.1111/1467-9280.00218