Individual differences in semantic priming and inhibitory control predict performance in the Remote Associates Test (RAT)

Thinking Skills and Creativity - Tập 50 - Trang 101426 - 2023
Raquel Lezama1, Carlos J. Gómez-Ariza2, M.Teresa Bajo1
1Department of Experimental Psychology, Mind, Brain and Behaviour Research Center (CIMCYC), University of Granada, Granada, Spain
2Department of Psychology, University of Jaén, Jaén, Spain

Tài liệu tham khảo

Abraham, 2014, Creative thinking as orchestrated by semantic processing vs. cognitive control brain networks, Frontiers in human neuroscience, 8, 1, 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00095 Abraham, 2012, Creativity and the brain: Uncovering the neural signature of conceptual expansion, Neuropsychologia, 50, 1906, 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.04.015 Alameda, 1995 Anaki, 2003, Is there a “strength effect” in automatic semantic priming?, Memory & Cognition, 31, 262, 10.3758/BF03194385 Anderson, 2021, Active forgetting: Adaptation of memory by prefrontal control, Annual Review of Psychology, 72, 1, 10.1146/annurev-psych-072720-094140 Bajo, 2006, Retrieval-induced forgetting in perceptually driven memory tests, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 1185 Bajo, 2021, Inhibitory control of information in memory across domains, Current Directions in Psychological Science, 30, 444, 10.1177/09637214211039857 Balota, 1986, Depth of automatic spreading activation: Mediated priming effects in pronunciation but not in lexical decision, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, memory, and cognition, 12, 336 Beaty, 2020, Default network contributions to episodic and semantic processing during divergent creative thinking: A representational similarity analysis, NeuroImage, 209, 1, 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116499 Beaty, 2022, Semantic memory and creativity: The costs and benefits of semantic memory structure in generating original ideas, Thinking & Reasoning, 29, 1 Beaty, 2019, Network neuroscience of creative cognition: Mapping cognitive mechanisms and individual differences in the creative brain, Current Opinion In Behavioral Sciences, 27, 22, 10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.08.013 Beaty, 2014, The roles of associative and executive processes in creative cognition, Memory Cognition, 42, 1186, 10.3758/s13421-014-0428-8 Benedek, 2023, The role of memory in creative ideation, Nature Reviews Psychology, 2, 246, 10.1038/s44159-023-00158-z Benedek, 2019, Toward a neurocognitive framework of creative cognition: The role of memory, attention, and cognitive control, Current Opinion in Behavioural Sciences, 27, 116, 10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.11.002 Benedek, 2012, Differential effects of cognitive inhibition and intelligence on creativity, Personality and individual differences, 53, 480, 10.1016/j.paid.2012.04.014 Benedek, 2018, Spontaneous and controlled processes in creative cognition, 285 Benedek, 2012, Associative abilities underlying creativity, Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 6, 273, 10.1037/a0027059 Benedek, 2013, Revisiting Mednick's model on creativity-related differences in associative hierarchies. Evidence for a common path to uncommon thought, The Journal of Creative Behaviour, 47, 273, 10.1002/jocb.35 Chiu, 2015, Improving your creative potential without awareness: Overinclusive thinking training, Thinking Skills and Creativity, 15, 1, 10.1016/j.tsc.2014.11.001 Chrysikou, 2019, Creativity in and out of (cognitive) control, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 27, 94, 10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.09.014 Collins, 1975, A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing, Psychological Review, 82, 407, 10.1037/0033-295X.82.6.407 Colzato, 2017, Prior meditation practice modulates performance and strategy use in convergent-and divergent-thinking problems, Mindfulness, 8, 10, 10.1007/s12671-014-0352-9 Cortes, 2019, Re-examining prominent measures of divergent and convergent creativity, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 27, 90, 10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.09.017 Cushen, 2018, Both attentional control and the ability to make remote associations aid spontaneous analogical transfer, Memory & Cognition, 46, 1398, 10.3758/s13421-018-0845-1 De Groot, 1983, The range of automatic spreading activation in word priming, Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22, 417, 10.1016/S0022-5371(83)90273-6 De Luca, 2022, The transfer of global and local processing modes, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 48, 1054 Diamond, 2013, Executive functions, Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135, 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750 Faul, 2009, Statistical power analyses using G* Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses, Behavior Research Methods, 41, 1149, 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149 Finke, 1992 Förster, 2010, GLOMOsys: A systems account of global versus local processing, Psychological Inquiry, 21, 175, 10.1080/1047840X.2010.487849 Förster, 2012, Sense creative! The impact of global and local vision, hearing, touching, tasting and smelling on creative and analytic thought, Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3, 108, 10.1177/1948550611410890 Friedman, 2002, The influence of approach and avoidance motor actions on creative cognition, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 41, 10.1006/jesp.2001.1488 Friedman, 2008, Activation and measurement of motivational states, 235 Gerver, 2023, Memory and creativity: A meta-analytic examination of the relationship between memory systems and creative cognition, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2023, 1 Goff, 2002 Gómez-Ariza, 2017, Memory inhibition as a critical factor preventing creative problem solving, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 43, 986 Gruszka, 2002, Priming and acceptance of close and remote associations by creative and less creative people, Creativity Research Journal, 14, 193, 10.1207/S15326934CRJ1402_6 Gupta, 2012, The road not taken: Creative solutions require avoidance of high-frequency responses, Psychological Science, 23, 288, 10.1177/0956797611429710 He, 2020, The relation between semantic memory structure, associative abilities, and verbal and figural creativity, Thinking & Reasoning, 27, 268, 10.1080/13546783.2020.1819415 Kenett, 2018, Investigating creativity from a semantic network perspective, 49 Kenett, 2014, Investigating the structure of semantic networks in low and high creative persons, Front. Hum. Neurosci., 8, 1, 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00407 Kenett, 2016, Structure and flexibility: Investigating the relation between the structure of the mental lexicon, fluid intelligence, and creative achievement, Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 10, 377, 10.1037/aca0000056 Kenett, 2017, The semantic distance task: Quantifying semantic distance with semantic network path length, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 43, 1470 Kumar, 2020, Distant connectivity and multiple-step priming in large-scale semantic networks, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 46, 2261 Lee, 2013, The cognitive underpinnings of creative thought: A latent variable analysis exploring the roles of intelligence and working memory in three creative thinking processes, Intelligence, 41, 306, 10.1016/j.intell.2013.04.008 Lerner, 2014, Integrating the automatic and the controlled: Strategies in semantic priming in an attractor network with latching dynamics, Cognitive Science, 38, 1562, 10.1111/cogs.12133 Levy, 2021, Unveiling the nature of interaction between semantics and phonology in lexical access based on multilayer networks, Scientific reports, 11, 14479, 10.1038/s41598-021-93925-y Li, 2021, Flexible semantic network structure supports the production of creative metaphor, Creativity Research Journal, 33, 209, 10.1080/10400419.2021.1879508 Liu, 2016, Broaden the mind before ideation: The effect of conceptual attention scope on creativity, Thinking Skills and Creativity, 22, 190, 10.1016/j.tsc.2016.10.004 Liu, 2018, Does scope of attention affect creativity? Testing the attentional priming hypothesis, The Journal of Creative Behavior, 54, 423, 10.1002/jocb.378 Luchini, 2023, Convergent thinking and insight problem solving relate to semantic memory network structure, Thinking Skills and Creativity, 48, 10.1016/j.tsc.2023.101277 Madore, 2015, Creativity and memory: Effects of an episodic-specificity induction on divergent thinking, Psychological Science, 26, 1461, 10.1177/0956797615591863 Madore, 2016, Divergent creative thinking in young and older adults: Extending the effects of an episodic specificity induction, Memory & Cognition, 44, 974, 10.3758/s13421-016-0605-z Marko, 2019, Remote associates test: An empirical proof of concept, Behavior Research Methods, 51, 2700, 10.3758/s13428-018-1131-7 Marko, 2021, The structure of semantic representation shapes controlled semantic retrieval, Memory (Hove, England), 29, 538, 10.1080/09658211.2021.1906905 Marron, 2018, Chain free association, creativity, and the default mode network, Neuropsychologia, 118, 40, 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.03.018 Martindale, 1995, Creativity and connectionism, 249 Martindale, 1999, Biological bases of creativity, 137 McRae, 2013, 14 Semantic Memory, 206 Mednick, 1962, The associative basis of the creative process, Psychological Review, 69, 220, 10.1037/h0048850 Mednick, 1968, The remote associates test, The Journal of Creative Behavior, 10.1002/j.2162-6057.1968.tb00104.x Mendelsohn, 1976, Associative and attentional processes in creative performance, Journal of Personality, 44, 341, 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1976.tb00127.x Michalko, 2023, Response modularity moderates how executive control aids fluent semantic memory retrieval, Memory (Hove, England), 1 Navon, 1977, Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception, Cognitive Psychology, 9, 353, 10.1016/0010-0285(77)90012-3 Neely, 1976, Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory: Evidence for facilitatory and inhibitory processes, Memory & Cognition, 4, 648, 10.3758/BF03213230 Nelson, 1993, Implicit memory: Effects of network size and interconnectivity on cued recall, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 747 Peláez-Alfonso, 2020, Normative data for 102 Spanish remote associate problems and age-related differences in performance, Psicológica Journal, 41, 39, 10.2478/psicolj-2020-0003 Radel, 2015, The role of (dis) inhibition in creativity: Decreased inhibition improves idea generation, Cognition, 134, 110, 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.09.001 Rivera, 2023, Second language acquisition of grammatical rules: The effects of learning condition, rule difficulty, and executive function, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1 Rossmann, 2010, Do creative people use shorter associative pathways?, Personality and Individual Differences, 49, 891, 10.1016/j.paid.2010.07.025 Sánchez-Casas, 2006, The nature of semantic priming: Effects of the degree of semantic similarity between primes and targets in Spanish, European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 18, 161, 10.1080/09541440500183830 Schilling, 2005, A" small-world" network model of cognitive insight, Creativity Research Journal, 17, 131, 10.1207/s15326934crj1702&3_2 Simon, 1985, What we know about the creative process, Frontiers in Creative and Innovative Management, 4, 3 Smith, 1991, Incubation and the persistence of fixation in problem solving, The American Journal of Psychology, 104, 61, 10.2307/1422851 Smith, 1993, Constraining effects of examples in a creative generation task, Memory & Cognition, 21, 837, 10.3758/BF03202751 Smith, 2013, Multiply-constrained semantic search in the Remote Associates Test, Cognition, 128, 64, 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.03.001 Sowden, 2019, The shifting sands of creative thinking: Connections to dual-process theory, 40 Storm, 2010, Overcoming fixation: Creative problem solving and retrieval-induced forgetting, Psychological Science, 21, 1263, 10.1177/0956797610379864 Storm, 2015, Mental fixation and metacognitive predictions of insight in creative problem solving, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 68, 802, 10.1080/17470218.2014.966730 Thakral, 2021, Linking creativity and false memory: Common consequences of a flexible memory system, Cognition, 217, 1, 10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104905 Topolinski, 2008, Where there's a will—There's no intuition. The unintentional basis of semantic coherence judgments, Journal of Memory and Language, 58, 1032, 10.1016/j.jml.2008.01.002 Valle, 2019, Inhibitory control during selective retrieval may hinder subsequent analogical thinking, PloS one, 14, 1, 10.1371/journal.pone.0211881 Valle, 2020, Cathodal transcranial direct current stimulation over the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex cancels out the cost of selective retrieval on subsequent analogical reasoning, Neuropsychologia, 141, 1, 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107431 Valle, 2020, Electrophysiological correlates of interference control at retrieval predict performance on a subsequent analogical reasoning task, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 173, 1, 10.1016/j.nlm.2020.107253 Volle, 2018, Associative and controlled cognition in divergent thinking: Theoretical, experimental, neuroimaging evidence, and new directions, 333 Wronska, 2018, Engaging in creativity broadens attentional scope, Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1, 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01772 Zabelina, 2016, Flexible or leaky attention in creative people? Distinct patterns of attention for different types of creative thinking, Memory & Cognition, 44, 488, 10.3758/s13421-015-0569-4 Zmigrod, 2015, Zooming into creativity: Individual differences in attentional global-local biases are linked to creative thinking, Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1, 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01647