How personal standards perfectionism and evaluative concerns perfectionism affect the error positivity and post-error behavior with varying stimulus visibility
Tóm tắt
Error detection is required in order to correct or avoid imperfect behavior. Although error detection is beneficial for some people, for others it might be disturbing. We investigated Gaudreau and Thompson’s (Personality and Individual Differences, 48, 532−537, 2010) model, which combines personal standards perfectionism (PSP) and evaluative concerns perfectionism (ECP). In our electrophysiological study, 43 participants performed a combination of a modified Simon task, an error awareness paradigm, and a masking task with a variation of stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA; 33, 67, and 100 ms). Interestingly, relative to low-ECP participants, high-ECP participants showed a better post-error accuracy (despite a worse classification accuracy) in the high-visibility SOA 100 condition than in the two low-visibility conditions (SOA 33 and SOA 67). Regarding the electrophysiological results, first, we found a positive correlation between ECP and the amplitude of the error positivity (Pe) under conditions of low stimulus visibility. Second, under the condition of high stimulus visibility, we observed a higher Pe amplitude for high-ECP–low-PSP participants than for high-ECP–high-PSP participants. These findings are discussed within the framework of the error-processing avoidance hypothesis of perfectionism (Stahl, Acharki, Kresimon, Völler, & Gibbons, International Journal of Psychophysiology, 97, 153–162, 2015).
Tài liệu tham khảo
Altstötter-Gleich, C., & Bergemann, N. (2006). Testgüte einer deutschsprachigen Version der Mehrdimensionalen Perfektionismus Skala von Frost, Marten, Lahart und Rosenblate (MPS-F). Diagnostica, 52, 105–18. doi:10.1026/0012-1924.52.3.105
Bartlett, F. C. (1958). Thinking: An experimental and social study. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Bode, S., Sewell, D. K., Lilburn, S., Forte, J. D., Smith, P. L., & Stahl, J. (2012). Predicting perceptual decision biases from early brain activity. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 12488–98. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1708-12.2012
Bode, S., & Stahl, J. (2014). Predicting errors from patterns of event-related potentials preceding an overt response. Biological Psychology, 103, 357–69. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.10.002
Burle, B., Vidal, F., Tandonnet, C., & Hasbroucq, T. (2004). Physiological evidence for response inhibition in choice reaction time tasks. Brain and Cognition, 56, 153–64. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2004.06.004
Danielmeier, C., & Ullsperger, M. (2011). Post-error adjustments. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 233. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00233
Dutilh, G., van Ravenzwaaij, D., Nieuwenhuis, S., van der Maas, H. J., Forstmann, B. U., & Wagenmakers, E.-J. (2012). How to measure post-error slowing: A confound and a simple solution. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 56, 208–16. doi:10.1016/j.jmp.2012.04.001
Falkenstein, M., Hohnsbein, J., Hoormann, J., & Blanke, L. (1991). Effects of crossmodal divided attention on late ERP components: II. Error processing in choice reaction tasks. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 78, 447–55. doi:10.1016/0013-4694(91)90062-9
Falkenstein, M., Hoormann, J., Christ, S., & Hohnsbein, J. (2000). ERP components on reaction errors and their functional significance: A tutorial. Biological Psychology, 51, 87–107. doi:10.1016/S0301-0511(99)00031-9
Frost, R. O., Marten, P., Lahart, C., & Rosenblate, R. (1990). The dimensions of perfectionism. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 14, 449–68. doi:10.1007/BF01172967
Gaudreau, P. (2013). The 2 × 2 model of perfectionism: Commenting the critical comments and suggestions of Stoeber (2012). Personality and Individual Differences, 55, 351–5. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2013.03.021
Gaudreau, P., & Thompson, A. (2010). Testing a 2 × 2 model of dispositional perfectionism. Personality and Individual Differences, 48, 532–7. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.11.031
Gaudreau, P., & Thompson, A. (2011). Erratum to “Testing a 2 × 2 model of dispositional perfectionism” [Personality and Individual Differences 48 (2010) 532–537]. Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 126. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2010.09.001
Gehring, W. J., Goss, B., Coles, M. G., Meyer, D. E., & Donchin, E. (1993). A neural system for error detection and compensation. Psychological Science, 4, 385–90. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1993.tb00586.x
Gibbons, H., Fritzsche, A.-S., Bienert, S., Armbrecht, A.-S., & Stahl, J. (2011). Percept-based and object-based error processing: An experimental dissociation of error-related negativity and error positivity. Clinical Neurophysiology, 122, 299–310. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2010.06.031
Gratton, G., Coles, M. G. H., & Donchin, E. (1983). A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 55, 468–84. doi:10.1016/0013-4694(83)90135-9
Green, D. M., & Swets, J. A. (1974). Signal detection theory and psychophysics (Revth ed.). Oxford, UK: Krieger.
Herrmann, M. J., Römmler, J., Ehlis, A.-C., Heidrich, A., & Fallgatter, A. J. (2004). Source localization (LORETA) of the errorrelated-negativity (ERN/Ne) and positivity (Pe). Cognitive Brain Research, 20, 294–9. doi:10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.02.013
Hewitt, P. L., & Flett, G. L. (1991). Perfectionism in the self and social contexts: Conceptualization, assessment, and association with psychopathology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 456–70. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.60.3.456
Koivula, N., Hassmén, P., & Fallby, J. (2002). Self-esteem and perfectionism in elite athletes: Effects on competitive anxiety and self-confidence. Personality and Individual Differences, 32, 865–75. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00092-7
Mathewson, K. J., Dywan, J., & Segalowitz, S. J. (2005). Brain bases of error-related ERPs as influenced by age and task. Biological Psychology, 70, 88–104. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2004.12.005
Moser, J. S., Moran, T. P., Schroder, H. S., Donnellan, M. B., & Yeung, N. (2013). On the relationship between anxiety and error monitoring: A meta-analysis and conceptual framework. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 466. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.0046
Nieuwenhuis, S., Ridderinkhof, K. R., Blom, J., Band, G. P. H., & Kok, A. (2001). Error-related brain potentials are differentially related to awareness of response errors: Evidence from an antisaccade task. Psychophysiology, 38, 752–60. doi:10.1017/S0048577201001111
O’Connell, R. G., Dockree, P. M., Bellgrove, M. A., Kelly, S. P., Hester, R., Garavan, H., … Foxe, J. J. (2007). The role of cingulate cortex in the detection of errors with and without awareness: A high-density electrical mapping study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 2571–9. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05477.x
Overbeek, T. J. M., Nieuwenhuis, S., & Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2005). Dissociable components of error processing: On the functional significance of the Pe vis-à-vis the ERN/Ne. Journal of Psychophysiology, 19, 319–29. doi:10.1027/0269-8803.19.4.319
Perrin, F., Pernier, J., Bertrand, O., & Echallier, J. F. (1989). Spherical splines for scalp potential and current density mapping. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 72, 184–7. doi:10.1016/0013-4694(89)90180-6
Pieters, G. L. M., de Bruijn, E. R. A., Maas, Y., Hulstijn, W., Vandereycken, W., Peuskens, J., & Sabbe, B. G. (2007). Action monitoring and perfectionism in anorexia nervosa. Brain and Cognition, 63, 42–50. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2006.07.009
Rabbitt, P. M. A. (1966). Errors and error correction in choice-response tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71, 264–72. doi:10.1037/h0022853
Schrijvers, D. L., De Bruijn, E. R. A., Destoop, M., Hulstijn, W., & Sabbe, B. G. C. (2010). The impact of perfectionism and anxiety traits on action monitoring in major depressive disorder. Journal of Neural Transmission, 117, 869–80. doi:10.1007/s00702-010-0419-2
Schroder, H. S., & Infantolino, Z. P. (2013). Distinguishing between types of errors and adjustments. Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 18356–7. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4078-13.2013
Shalgi, S., Barkan, I., & Deouell, L. Y. (2009). On the positive side of error processing: Error-awareness positivity revisited. European Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 1522–32. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06690.x
Simon, J. R. (1969). Reaction toward the source of stimulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 174–6. doi:10.1037/h0027448
Stahl, J., Acharki, M., Kresimon, M., Völler, F., & Gibbons, H. (2015). Perfect error processing: Perfectionism-related variations in action monitoring and error processing mechanisms. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 97, 153–62. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.06.002
Steinhauser, M., & Yeung, N. (2010). Decision processes in human performance monitoring. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 15643–53. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1899-10.2010
Steinhauser, M., & Yeung, N. (2012). Error awareness as evidence accumulation: Effects of speed–accuracy trade-off on error signaling. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 240. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2012.00240
Tops, M., Koole, S. L., & Wijers, A. A. (2013). The Pe of perfectionism. Concern over mistakes predicts the amplitude of a late frontal error positivity. Journal of Psychophysiology, 27, 84–94. doi:10.1027/0269-8803/a000090
Vidal, F., Burle, B., Bonnet, M., Grapperon, J., & Hasbroucq, T. (2003). Error negativity on correct trials: A reexamination of available data. Biological Psychology, 64, 265–82. doi:10.1016/S0301-0511(03)00097-8
Vidal, F., Hasbroucq, T., Grapperon, J., & Bonnet, M. (2000). Is the “error negativity” specific to errors? Biological Psychology, 51, 109–28. doi:10.1016/S0301-0511(99)00032-0
Wang, K. T. (2012). Personal and family perfectionism of Taiwanese college students: Relationships with depression, self-esteem, achievement motivation, and academic grades. International Journal of Psychology, 47, 305–14. doi:10.1080/00207594.2011.626050
Wessel, J. R. (2012). Error awareness and the error-related negativity: Evaluating the first decade of evidence. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 88. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2012.00088