Hearing hooves, thinking zebras: A review of the inverse base-rate effect
Tóm tắt
People often fail to use base-rate information appropriately in decision-making. This is evident in the inverse base-rate effect, a phenomenon in which people tend to predict a rare outcome for a new and ambiguous combination of cues. While the effect was first reported in 1988, it has recently seen a renewed interest from researchers concerned with learning, attention and decision-making. However, some researchers have raised concerns that the effect arises in specific circumstances and is unlikely to provide insight into general learning and decision-making processes. In this review, we critically evaluate the evidence for and against the main explanations that have been proposed to explain the effect, and identify where this evidence is currently weak. We argue that concerns about the effect are not well supported by the data. Instead, the evidence supports the conclusion that the effect is a result of general mechanisms that provides a useful opportunity to understand the processes involved in learning and decision making. We discuss gaps in our knowledge and some promising avenues for future research, including the relevance of the effect to models of attentional change in learning, an area where the phenomenon promises to contribute new insights.
Từ khóa
Tài liệu tham khảo
Bar-Hillel, M. (1980). The base-rate fallacy in probability judgments. Acta Psychologica, 44, 211–33.
Bar-Hillel, M., & Fischhoff, B. (1981). When do base rates affect predictions? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 671–80.
Binder, A., & Estes, W. K. (1966). Transfer of response in visual recognition situations as a function of frequency variables. Psychological Monographs, 80 (23, Whole No. 631).
Blanco, F., Baeyens, F., & Beckers, T. (2014). Blocking in human causal learning is affected by outcome assumptions manipulated through causal structure. Learning & Behavior, 42, 185–199.
Blanco, F., Matute, H., & Vadillo, M. A. (2013). Interactive effects of the probability of the cue and the probability of the outcome on the overestimation of null contingency. Learning & Behavior, 41, 333–340.
Blanco, F., & Matute, H. (2019). Base-rate expectations modulate the causal illusion. PloS one, 14, e0212615.
Bohil, C. J., Markman, A. B., & Maddox, T. (2005). A feature-salience analogue of the inverse base-rate effect. The Korean Journal of Thinking & Problem Solving, 15, 17–28.
Burling, J. M., & Yoshida, H. (2016). Highlighting in early childhood: Learning biases through attentional shifting. Cognitive Science, 41, 96–119.
Butt, J. (1988). Frequency judgments in an auditing-related task. Journal of Accounting Research, 26, 315–30.
Casscells, W., Schoenberger, A., & Graboys, T. B. (1978). Interpretation by physicians of clinical laboratory results. New England Journal of Medicine, 299, 999–1001.
Christensen-Szalanski, J. J. J. & Beach, L. R. (1982) Experience and the base-rate fallacy. Organization Behavior and Human Performance, 29, 270-78.
Christensen-Szalanski, J. J. J. & Bushyhead, J. B. (1981). Physicians' use of probabilistic information in a real clinical setting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 928–35.
Dennis, S., & Kruschke, J. K. (1998). Shifting attention in cued recall. Australian Journal of Psychology, 50, 131-138.
Denton, S. E., & Kruschke, J. K. (2006). Attention and salience in associative blocking. Learning & Behavior, 34, 285–304.
Dickinson, A., & Burke, J. (1996). Within-compound associations mediate the retrospective revaluation of causality judgements. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 37, 397–416.
Dickinson, A., Shanks, D. R., & Evenden, J. L. (1984). Judgement of act-outcome contingency: The role of selective attribution. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 36A, 29-50.
Don, H. J., & Livesey, E. J. (2017). Effects of outcome and trial frequency on the inverse base-rate effect. Memory & cognition, 45, 493–507.
Don, H. J. & Livesey, E. J. (2018). Is the blocking effect sensitive to causal model? It depends how you ask. Proceedings of the 40th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp.1633–1638). Madison: Cognitive Science Society
Don, H. J. & Livesey, E. J. (2021). Attention biases in the inverse base-rate effect persist into new learning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1177/1747021820985522.
Don, H. J., Goldwater, M. B., Otto, A. R., & Livesey, E. J. (2015). Connecting ruleabstraction and model-based choice across disparate learning tasks. In P. Bello, M. Guarini, M. McShane, & B. Scassellati (Eds.), Proceedings of the 36th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 590–595). Pasadena, California: Cognitive Science Society.
Don, H. J., Goldwater, M. B., Otto, A. R., & Livesey, E. J. (2016). Rule abstraction, model-based choice, and cognitive reflection. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23, 1615–1623.
Don, H. J., Beesley, T., & Livesey, E. J. (2019a). Learned predictiveness models predict opposite attention biases in the inverse base-rate effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition, 45, 143.
Don, H. J., Otto, A. R., Cornwall, A. C., Davis, T., & Worthy, D. A. (2019b). Learning reward frequency over reward probability: A tale of two learning rules. Cognition, 193, 104042.
Don, H. J., Goldwater, M. B., Greenaway, J. K., Hutchings, R., & Livesey, E. J. (2020). Relational rule discovery in complex discrimination learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 46, 1807–1827.
Fagot, J., Kruschke, J. K., Dépy, D., & Vauclair, J. (1998). Associative learning in baboons (Papio papio) and humans (Homo sapiens): Species differences in learned attention to visual features. Animal Cognition, 1, 123–133.
Gluck, M. A. (1992). Stimulus sampling and distributed representation in adaptive network theories of learning. In A. F. Healy, S. M. Kosslyn, & R. M. Shiffrin (Eds.), Essays in honor of William K. Estes, Vol. 1. From learning theory to connectionist theory; Vol. 2. From learning processes to cognitive processes (pp. 169–199). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Gluck, M. A., & Bower, G. H. (1988). From conditioning to category learning: An adaptive network model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117, 227–247.
Goldwater, M. B., Don, H. J., Krusche, M., & Livesey, E. J. (2018). Relational discovery in category learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General.
Granger, K. T., Moran, P. M., Buckley, M. G., & Haselgrove, M. (2016). Enhanced latent inhibition in high schizotypy individuals. Personality and Individual differences, 91, 31-39.
Hamilton, D. L. (Ed.) (1981). Cognitive processes in stereotyping and intergroup behavior. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
Haselgrove, M., Le Pelley, M. E., Singh, N. K., Teow, H. Q., Morris, R. W., Green, M. J., ... & Killcross, S. (2016). Disrupted attentional learning in high schizotypy: Evidence of aberrant salience. British journal of psychology, 107, 601–624.
Inkster, A., Milton, F., Edmunds, C. E. R., Benattayallah, A., & Wills, A. (2019a) Neural Correlates of the Inverse Base Rate Effect. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/muqrh
Inkster, A., Mitchell, C., Schlegelmilch, R., & Wills, A. (2019b). Effect of a context shift on the inverse base rate effect. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/rpb7x
Johansen, M. K., Fouquet, N., & Shanks, D. R. (2007). Paradoxical effects of base rates and representation in category learning. Memory & Cognition, 35, 1365–1379.
Johansen, M. K., Fouquet, N., & Shanks, D. R. (2010). Featural selective attention, exemplar representation, and the inverse base-rate effect. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17, 637–643.
Jones, P. M., Zaksaite, T., & Mitchell, C. J. (2019). Uncertainty and blocking in human causal learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition, 45, 111.
Juslin, P., Wennerholm, P., & Winman, A. (2001). High-level reasoning and base-rate use: Do we need cue-competition to explain the inverse base-rate effect? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 849–871.
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1973). On the psychology of prediction. Psychological review, 80, 237–251.
Kalish, M. L. (2001). An inverse base rate effect with continuously valued stimuli. Memory & Cognition, 29, 4, 587–597.
Kalish, M. L., & Kruschke, J. K. (2000). The role of attention shifts in the categorization of continuous dimensioned stimuli. Psychological Research, 64, 105–116.
Kamin, L.J. (1969). Selective association and conditioning. In N.J. Mackintosh & W.K. Honig (Eds.), Fundamental issues in associative learning (pp. 42–64). Halifax: Dalhousie University Press.
Koehler, J. J. (1996). The base rate fallacy reconsidered: Descriptive, normative, and methodological challenges. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 19, 1–53.
Kruschke, J. K. (1996). Base rates in category learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 3–26.
Kruschke, J. K. (2001a). The inverse base-rate effect is not explained by eliminative inference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 1385–1400.
Kruschke, J. K. (2001b). Toward a unified model of attention in associative learning. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 45, 812–863.
Kruschke, J. K. (2003). Attentional theory is a viable explanation of the inverse base rate effect: A reply to Winman, Wennerholm, and Juslin (2003). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 1396–1400.
Kruschke, J. K. (2005). Learning involves attention. In G. Houghton (Ed.), Connectionist models in cognitive psychology (pp. 113–140). Hove, East Sussex, UK: Psychology Press.
Kruschke, J. K. (2009). Highlighting: A canonical experiment. In B. H. Ross (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 51, pp.153–185).
Kruschke, J. K., Kappenman, E. S., & Hetrick, W. P. (2005). Eye gaze and individual differences consistent with learned attention in associative blocking and highlighting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 830-845.
Kutzner, F. L., & Fiedler, K. (2015). No correlation, no evidence for attention shift in category learning: Different mechanisms behind illusory correlations and the inverse base-rate effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144, 58.
Lamberts, K., & Kent, C. (2007). No evidence for rule-based processing in the inverse base-rate effect. Memory & Cognition, 35, 2097–2105.
Larkin, M. J., Aitken, M. R., & Dickinson, A. (1998). Retrospective revaluation of causal judgments under positive and negative contingencies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 1331.
Lee, J. C., Hayes, B. K., & Lovibond, P. F. (2018). Peak shift and rules in human generalization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 44, 1955–1970.
Le Pelley, M. E. (2004). The role of associative history in models of associative learning: A selective review and a hybrid model. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section B, 57, 193–243.
Le Pelley, M. E., & McLaren, I. P. L. (2003). Learned associability and associative change in human causal learning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: B, Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 56, 68–79.
Le Pelley, M. E., Mitchell, C. J., Beesley, T., George, D. N., & Wills, A. J. (2016, August 8). Attention and associative learning in humans: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin 142, 1111–1140.
Le Pelley, M. E., Schmidt-Hansen, M., Harris, N. J., Lunter, C. M., & Morris, C. S. (2010). Disentangling the attentional deficit in schizophrenia: Pointers from schizotypy. Psychiatry Research, 176, 143–149.
Little, J. L., & McDaniel, M. A. (2015). Individual differences in category learning: Memorization versus rule abstraction. Memory & cognition, 43, 283–297.
Livesey, E. J., Greenaway, J., Schubert, S., & Thorwart, A. (2019). Testing the deductive inferential account of blocking in causal learning. Memory & Cognition, 47, 1120–1132.
Livesey, E., Lee, J., Shone, L. (2013). The relationship between blocking and inference in causal learning. 35th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (COGSCI 2013), Austin: Cognitive Science Society.
Lochmann, T., & Wills, A. J. (2003). Predictive history in an allergy prediction task. In Proceedings of EuroCogSci (Vol. 3, pp. 217–222).
Luque, D., Vadillo, M. A., Gutiérrez-Cobo, M. J., & Le Pelley, M. E. (2016). The blocking effect in associative learning involves learned biases in rapid attentional capture. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1–26.
Luque, D., Cobos, P. L., & López, F. J. (2008). Interference between cues requires a causal scenario: Favorable evidence for causal reasoning models in learning processes. Learning and Motivation, 39(3), 196–208.
Mackintosh, N. J. (1975). A theory of attention: Variations in the associability of stimuli with reinforcement. Psychological Review, 82, 276–298.
Manis, M., Dovalina, I., Avis, N. E., & Cardoze, S. (1980). Base rates can affect individual predictions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38(2), 231248.
Markman, A. B. (1989). LMS rules and the inverse base-rate effect: Comment on Gluck and Bower (1988). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118, 417–421.
Matute, H., Blanco, F., Yarritu, I., Díaz-Lago, M., Vadillo, M. A., & Barberia, I. (2015). Illusions of causality: how they bias our everyday thinking and how they could be reduced. Frontiers in psychology, 6, 888.
McDaniel, M. A., Cahill, M. J., Robbins, M., & Wiener, C. (2014). Individual differences in learning and transfer: Stable tendencies for learning exemplars versus abstracting rules. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 668–693.
McLaren, I. P., Forrest, C. L. D., McLaren, R. P., Jones, F. W., Aitken, M. R. F., & Mackintosh, N. J. (2014). Associations and propositions: The case for a dualprocess account of learning in humans. Neurobiology of learning and memory, 108, 185–195.
Medin, D. L., & Bettger, J. G., (1991). Sensitivity to changes in base-rate information. The American Journal of Psychology, 104, 311–332.
Medin, D. L., & Edelson, S. M., (1988). Problem structure and the use of base-rate information from experience. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 1, 68–85.
Medin, D. L., & Schaffer, M. M. (1978). Context theory of classification learning. Psychological review, 85, 207.
Mitchell, C. J., & Le Pelley, M. E. (Eds.). (2010). Attention and associative learning: From brain to behaviour. Oxford University Press, USA.
Mitchell, C. J., Lovibond, P. F., & Gan, C. Y. (2005). A dissociation between causal judgment and outcome recall. Psychonomic bulletin & review, 12, 950–954.
Mitchell, C. J., Lovibond, P. F., Minard, E., & Lavis, Y. (2006). Forward blocking in human learning sometimes reflects the failure to encode a cue–outcome relationship. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 830-844.
Mitchell, C. J., Griffiths, O., Seetoo, J., & Lovibond, P. F. (2012). Attentional mechanisms in learned predictiveness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 38, 191.
Nosofsky, R. M. (1984). Choice, similarity, and the context theory of classification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, memory, and cognition, 10, 104–114.
Nosofsky, R. M. (1986). Attention, similarity, and the identification–categorization relationship. Journal of experimental psychology: General, 115, 39.
O'Bryan, S. R., Worthy, D. A., Livesey, E. J., & Davis, T. (2018). Model-based fMRI reveals dissimilarity processes underlying base rate neglect. Elife, 7, e36395.
Paskewitz, S., & Jones, M. (2020). Dissecting EXIT. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 97, 102371.
Pearce, J. M., & Hall, G. (1980). A model for Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of conditioned but not of unconditioned stimuli. Psychological Review, 87, 532–552.
Pearce, J. M., & Mackintosh, N. J. (2010). Two theories of attention: A review and a possible integration. Attention and associative learning: From brain to behaviour, 11–39.
Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 3–25.
Rescorla, R. A., & Wagner, A. R. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In A. H. Black & W. F. Prokasy (Eds.). Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory (pp. 64–99). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Rosenblatt, F. (1961). Principles of neurodynamics. perceptrons and the theory of brain mechanisms (No. VG-1196-G-8). Cornell Aeronautical Lab Inc Buffalo NY.
Shanks, D. R. (1992) Connectionist accounts of the inverse base-rate effect in categorization. Connection Science, 4, 3-18.
Shanks, D. R., & Darby, R. J. (1998). Feature-and rule-based generalization in human associative learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 24, 405–415.
Sherman, J. W., Kruschke, J. K., Sherman, S. J., Percy, E. J., Petrocelli, J. V., & Conrey, F. R. (2009). Attentional processes in stereotype formation: a common model for category accentuation and illusory correlation. Journal of personality and social psychology, 96, 305–323.
Stewart, N., & Morin, C. (2007). Dissimilarity is used as evidence of category membership in multidimensional perceptual categorization: A test of the similarity–dissimilarity generalized context model. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 1337–1346.
Sutherland, N. S., & Mackintosh, N. J. (1971). Mechanisms of animal discrimination learning. New York: Academic Press.
Thorwart, A., & Livesey, E. J. (2016). Three ways that non-associative knowledge may affect associative learning processes. Frontiers in psychology, 7, 2024.
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1973). Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency and probability. Cognitive psychology, 5, 207–232.
Tversky, A. & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185, 1124–1131.
Van Hamme, L. J., & Wasserman, E. A. (1994). Cue competition in causality judgments: The role of nonpresentation of compound stimulus elements. Learning and motivation, 25, 127–151.
Waldmann, M. R. (2000). Competition among causes but not effects in predictive and diagnostic learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 53–76.
Waldmann, M. R. (2001). Predictive versus diagnostic causal learning: Evidence from an overshadowing paradigm. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 600–608.
Waldmann, M. R., & Holyoak, K. J. (1992). Predictive and diagnostic learning within causal models: Asymmetries in cue competition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121, 222–236.
Widrow, B., & Hoff, M.E. (1960). Adaptive switching circuits. 1960 WESCON Convention Record Part IV, 96–104.
Wills, A. J., Barrasin, T. J, & McLaren, I. P. L. (2011a). Working memory capacity and generalization in predictive learning. In L. Carlson, C. Hölscher, & T. Shipley (Eds.), Proceedings of the 33rd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 3205–3210). Austin: Cognitive Science Society.
Wills, A. J., Graham, S., Koh, Z., McLaren, I. P., & Rolland, M. D. (2011b). Effects of concurrent load on feature-and rule-based generalization in human contingency learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 37(3), 308–316.
Wills, A. J., Lavric, A., Hemmings, Y., Surrey, E. (2014). Attention, predictive learning, and the inverse base-rate effect: Evidence from event-related potentials. Neuroimage, 87, 61–71.
Winman, A., Wennerholm, P. & Juslin, P. (2003). Can attentional theory explain the inverse base rate effect? Comment on Kruschke (2001). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 1390–1395.
Winman, A., Wennerholm, P., Juslin, P. & Shanks, D. R. (2005). Evidence for rule-based processes in the inverse base-rate effect. The Quarterly Journal Of Experimental Psychology, 58A, 789–815.
Wood, M. J. (2009). Categorization of partially occluded visual stimuli: bridging the gap between completion and classification (Doctoral dissertation, Dept. of Psychology-Simon Fraser University).
Wood, M. J., & Blair, M. R. (2011). Informed inferences of unknown feature values in categorization. Memory & cognition, 39, 666–674.