From Vision to Action and Action to Vision: A Convergent Route Approach to Vision, Action, and Attention

Psychology of Learning and Motivation - Tập 42 - Trang 225-264 - 2003
Glyn W. Humphreys, M. Jane Riddoch

Tài liệu tham khảo

Ackroyd, 2002, Widening the sphere of influence. Using a tool to extend extrapersonal visual space in a patient with severe neglect, Neurocase, 8, 1, 10.1093/neucas/8.1.1 Aglioti, 1995, Size contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand, Current Biology, 5, 679, 10.1016/S0960-9822(95)00133-3 Arbib, 1997, Modelling visuomotor transformations, Vol. II, 65 Beauvois, 1982, Optic aphasia: A process of interaction between vision and language, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 289, 35, 10.1098/rstb.1982.0070 Bekkering, 2002, Visual search is modulated by action-intention, Psychological Science, 13, 370, 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2002.00466.x Berti, 2000, When far becomes near: Remapping of space by tool use, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 415, 10.1162/089892900562237 Bonda, 1996, Specific involvement of human parietal systems and the amygdala in the perception of biological motion, Journal of Neuroscience, 16, 3737, 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-11-03737.1996 Bonfiglioli, 2002, Action and perception: Evidence against converging selection processes, Visual Cognition, 9, 458, 10.1080/13506280143000539 Boutsen, L., & Humphreys, G. W. (2002). On the interaction between perceptual and response selection: Evidence from a patient with frontal lobe damage. Neuropsychologia Bridgeman, 2002, Attention and visually guided behaviour in distinct systems, 120 Bundesen, 1990, A theory of visual attention, Psychological Review, 49, 113 Caramazza, 1990, Multiple semantics or multiple confusions?, Cognitive Neuropsychology, 7, 161, 10.1080/02643299008253441 Chainay, 2002, Privileged access to action from objects relative to words, Psychological Bulletin and Review, 9, 348, 10.3758/BF03196292 Chainay, 2002, Neuropsychological evidence for a convergent route model for action, Cognitive Neuropsychology, 19, 67, 10.1080/02643290143000097 Chelazzi, 1993, A neural basis for visual search in inferior temporal cortex, Nature, 363, 345, 10.1038/363345a0 Coltheart, 1978, Lexical access in simple reading tasks, 151 Coslett, 1992, Optic aphasia and the right hemisphere: A replication and extension, Brain and Language, 43, 148, 10.1016/0093-934X(92)90026-B Damasio, 1990, Category-related recognition defects as a clue to the neural substrates of knowledge, Trends in the Neurosciences, 13, 95, 10.1016/0166-2236(90)90184-C Decety, 1997, Brain activity during observation of actions: Influence of action content and subject's strategy, Brain, 120, 1763, 10.1093/brain/120.10.1763 Decety, 1994, Mapping motor representations with PET, Nature, 371, 600, 10.1038/371600a0 Della Sala, 1991, Right-sided anarchic (alien) hand: A longitudinal study, Neuropsychologia, 29, 1113, 10.1016/0028-3932(91)90081-I De Renzi, 1982, Modality-specific and supramodal mechanisms of apraxia, Brain, 105, 301, 10.1093/brain/105.2.301 Deubel, 1996, Saccade target selection and object recognition: Evidence for a common attentional mechanism, Vision Research, 36, 1827, 10.1016/0042-6989(95)00294-4 Deubel, 1998, Selective dorsal and visual processing: Evidence for a common attentional mechanism, Visual Cognition, 5, 1827, 10.1080/713756776 Duncan, 1984, Selective attention and the organisation of visual information, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 10, 501, 10.1037/0096-3445.113.4.501 Duncan, 1996, Cooperating brain systems in selective perception and action, 549 Duncan, 1989, Visual search and stimulus similarity, Psychological Review, 96, 433, 10.1037/0033-295X.96.3.433 Ellis, 2000, Micro-affordance: The potentiation of components of action by seen objects, British Journal of Psychology, 91, 451, 10.1348/000712600161934 Franz, 2000, Grasping visual illusions: No evidence for a dissociation between perception and action, Psychological Science, 11, 20, 10.1111/1467-9280.00209 Freund, 1889, Über optische aphasie and seelenblindheit, Archiv für Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten, 20, 371, 10.1007/BF02086691 Garrard, 2002, Semantic dementia: A category-specific paradox, 149 Geschwind, 1965, Disconnection syndromes in animals and man, Brain, 88, 237, 10.1093/brain/88.2.237 Gibson, 1979 Grabowski, 1998, Premotor and prefrontal correlates of category-related lexical retrieval, Neuroimage, 7, 232, 10.1006/nimg.1998.0324 Grafton, 1997, Premotor cortex activation during observation and naming of familiar tools, Neuroimage, 6, 231, 10.1006/nimg.1997.0293 Grèzes, 1998, Top-down effects of strategy on the perception of human biological motion: A PET investigation, Cognitive Neuropsychology, 15, 553, 10.1080/026432998381023 Grèzes, 1999, The effects of learning and intention on the neural network involved in the perception of meaningless actions, Brain, 122, 1875, 10.1093/brain/122.10.1875 Grèzes, 2002, Does visual perception of object afford action? Evidence from a neuroimaging study, Neuropsychologia, 40, 212, 10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00089-6 Haffenden, 1998, The effect of pictorial illusion on prehension and perception, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 122, 10.1162/089892998563824 Heinke, 2003, Attention, spatial representation and visual neglect, Psychological Review, 110, 29, 10.1037/0033-295X.110.1.29 Hillis, 1995, Cognitive and neural mechanisms underlying visual and semantic processing: Implications from “optic aphasia.”, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, 457, 10.1162/jocn.1995.7.4.457 Hodges, 2000, The role of conceptual knowledge in object use: Evidence from semantic dementia, Brain, 123, 1913, 10.1093/brain/123.9.1913 Hodges, 1999, ‘What’ and ‘how’: Evidence for the dissociation of object knowledge and mechanical problem solving skills in the human brain, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 96, 9444, 10.1073/pnas.96.16.9444 Hommel, 2000, The prepared reflex: Automaticity and control in stimulus-response translation, 247 Humphreys, 1998, Neural representation of objects in space, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 353, 1341, 10.1098/rstb.1998.0288 Humphreys, 2000, One more cup of coffee for the road: Object-action assemblies, response blocking and response capture after frontal lobe damage, Experimental Brain Research, 133, 81, 10.1007/s002210000403 Humphreys, 2001, Detection by action: Neuropsychological evidence for action-defined templates in visual search, Nature Neuroscience, 4, 84, 10.1038/82940 Iriki, 1996, Coding of modified body schema during tool use by macaque postcentral neurones, NeuroReport, 7, 2325, 10.1097/00001756-199610020-00010 Karnath, 1988, Deficits of attention in acute and recovered visual hemi-neglect, Neuropsychologia, 26, 27, 10.1016/0028-3932(88)90028-0 Kumada, 2001, Lexical recovery from extinction: Interactions between visual form and stored knowledge modulate visual selection, Cognitive Neuropsychology, 18, 465, 10.1080/02643290042000224 Lauro-Grotto, 1997, Modality-specific operations in semantic dementia, Cortex, 33, 593, 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70720-2 Lhermitte, 1983, ‘Utilisation’ behaviour and its relations to lesions of the frontal lobes, Brain, 106, 237, 10.1093/brain/106.2.237 Lhermitte, 1973, A visual-speech disconnection syndrome. Report of a case with optic aphasia, Brain, 96, 695, 10.1093/brain/96.4.695 Liepmann, 1905, Die linke hemisphaere und das handeln, Muenchner Medizinische Wochenschrift, 49, 2322 Malach, 1995, Object-related activity revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging in human occipital cortex, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 92, 8135, 10.1073/pnas.92.18.8135 Manning, 1992, Optic aphasia with spared action naming: A description and possible loci of impairment, Neuropsychologia, 30, 587, 10.1016/0028-3932(92)90061-P Martin, 1995, Discrete cortical regions associated with knowledge of colour and knowledge of action, Science, 270, 102, 10.1126/science.270.5233.102 Mattingley, 1997, Pre-attentive filling in of visual surfaces in parietal extinction, Science, 275, 671, 10.1126/science.275.5300.671 Meegan, 1999, Visual search and target-directed action, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1347, 10.1037/0096-1523.25.5.1347 Milner, 1995 Milner, 1999, A paradoxical improvement of misreaching in optic ataxia: Evidence for two separate neural systems for visual localisation, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 266, 2225, 10.1098/rspb.1999.0912 Milner, 1991, Perception and action in ‘visual form agnosia.’, Brain, 114, 405, 10.1093/brain/114.1.405 Passingham, 1998, How do visual instructions influence the motor system?, Novartis Foundation Symposium, 218, 129 Pavani, 1999, Are perception and action affected differently by the Titchener circle illusion?, Experimental Brain Research, 127, 95, 10.1007/s002210050777 Pavese, 2002, Action matters: The role of action plans and object affordances in selection for action, Visual Cognition, 9, 559, 10.1080/13506280143000584 Perenin, 1988, Optic ataxia: A specific disruption in visuomotor mechanisms. 1. Different aspects of the deficit in reaching for objects, Brain, 111, 643, 10.1093/brain/111.3.643 Petersen, 1988, Positron emission tomographic studies of the cortical anatomy of single word processing, Nature, 331, 585, 10.1038/331585a0 Phillips, 2002, The neural substrates of action retrieval: An examination of semantic and visual routes to action, Visual Cognition, 9, 662, 10.1080/13506280143000610 Phillips, J. A., Noppeney, U., Humphreys, G. W., & Price, C. J. (in press). To what extent can category-specific deficits be explained by a differentiated semantic system? A PET study. Brain Phillips, 2002, S-R correspondence effects of irrelevant visual affordance: Time course and specificity of response activation, Visual Cognition, 9, 540, 10.1080/13506280143000575 Pilgrim, 1991, Impairment of action to visual objects in a case of ideomotor apraxia, Cognitive Neuropsychology, 8, 459, 10.1080/02643299108253383 Pisella, 2000, An “automatic pilot” for the hand in posterior parietal cortex. Toward reinterpreting optic ataxia, Nature Neuroscience, 3, 729, 10.1038/76694 Posner, 1990, The attention system of the human brain, Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 25, 10.1146/annurev.ne.13.030190.000325 Potter, 1975, Time to understand pictures and words, Nature, 253, 437, 10.1038/253437a0 Reason, 1984, Lapses of attention Riddoch, 1998, An experimental study of anarchic hand syndrome: Evidence that visual affordances direct action, Cognitive Neuropsychology, 15, 645, 10.1080/026432998381041 Riddoch, 1987, A case of integrative agnosia, Brain, 110, 1431, 10.1093/brain/110.6.1431 Riddoch, 1987, Visual object processing in optic aphasia: A case of semantic access agnosia, Cognitive Neuropsychology, 4, 131, 10.1080/02643298708252038 Riddoch, 2000, The Neuropsychology of object recognition, 45 Riddoch, 2000, Neuropsychological evidence distinguishing object selection from action (effector) selection, Cognitive Neuropsychology, 17, 547, 10.1080/02643290050110647 Riddoch, 2000, Visual affordances and object selection, 603 Riddoch, 2001, An experimental analysis of anarchic lower limb action, Neuropsychologia, 39, 574, 10.1016/S0028-3932(00)00152-4 Riddoch, 2003, Seeing the action: Neuropsychologial evidence for action-based effects on object selection, Nature Neuroscience, 6, 82, 10.1038/nn984 Riddoch, 2002, Dissociations between object knowledge and everyday action, Neurocase, 8, 100, 10.1093/neucas/8.1.100 Riddoch, 1989, Routes to action: Evidence from apraxia, Cognitive Neuropsychology, 6, 437, 10.1080/02643298908253424 Rossetti, 2002, Several different “vision for action” systems: A guide to dissociating and integrating dorsal and ventral functions, 62 Rothi, 1997 Rumiati, 1998, Recognition by action: Dissociating visual and semantic routes to action in normal observers, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 631, 10.1037/0096-1523.24.2.631 Schneider, 2002, Selection-or-perception and selection-for-spatial-motor-action are coupled by visual attention: A review of recent findings and new evidence from stimulus-driven saccade control, 609 Snowden, 1989, Semantic dementia: A form of circumscribed cerebral atrophy, Behavioural Neurology, 2, 167, 10.1155/1989/124043 Tipper, 1998, Action based mechanisms of attention, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 353, 1385, 10.1098/rstb.1998.0292 Tipper, 1997, Selective reaching to grasp: Evidence for distractor interference effects, Visual Cognition, 4, 1, 10.1080/713756749 Tipper, 1992, Selective reaching: Evidence for action-entered attention, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 891, 10.1037/0096-1523.18.4.891 Tipper, 2002, Action-centred negative priming, Visual Cognition, 9, 591, 10.1080/13506280143000593 Tucker, 1998, On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 830, 10.1037/0096-1523.24.3.830 Tucker, 2001, The potentiation of grasp types during visual object categorisation, Visual Cognition, 8, 769, 10.1080/13506280042000144 Ungerleider, 1982, Two cortical visual systems, 549 Vitkovitch, 1991, Perseverant responding in speeded picture naming: Its in the links, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 17, 664, 10.1037/0278-7393.17.4.664 Warburton, 1996, Noun and verb retrieval by normal subjects, Brain, 119, 159, 10.1093/brain/119.1.159 Warrington, 1984, Category-specific semantic impairments, Brain, 107, 829, 10.1093/brain/107.3.829 Yoon, 2002, Modelling direct perceptual constraints on action selection: The Naming and Action Model (NAM), Visual Cognition, 9, 615, 10.1080/13506280143000601