Follow the Leader: Parent- and Child-led Synchrony in Competitive and Cooperative play

Jessica Yarmolovsky1, Ronny Geva2,1,3
1The Department of Psychology, Bar Ilan University, Ramat Gan, Israel
2Department of Psychology, The Gonda Multidisciplinary Brain Research Center, Bar Ilan University, Ramat Gan, Israel
3The Developmental Neuropsychology Lab, The Susan Gonda Brain Research Center, Bar Ilan University, Ramat Gan, Israel

Tóm tắt

AbstractSocial interactions involve both cooperation to achieve a shared goal and competition over shared resources and rewards. The ability to engage in inter-personal coordination is an important measure of socio-emotional and cognitive well-being. Both cooperation and competition require interpersonal coordination, however with different motivational backgrounds. Competition is defined by a higher level of extrinsic motivation, while cooperation is related to more intrinsic motivation. In the context of the parent-child dyad, each individual has different motivations and contributions to the dyad. The parent’s and child’s sense of competitiveness and contribution to inter-personal synchrony will presumably differ from each other and adapt to one another. The current research employed Motion Energy Analysis, an objective measure of coordination of movements between individuals, to measure motor in-phase and anti-phase synchrony during parent-child cooperative and competitive play, with a focus on parent and child-led synchrony. Findings highlight that parents rate themselves as less competitive than their children rate themselves; with no such difference noted in cooperation. Further, parent-led motor synchrony is defined more by in-phase coordination in competition, especially when the interaction is novel. Alternatively, child-led motor synchrony is more anti-phase during competition. In cooperation parents and children lead synchrony to the same extent and in the same phase. Current findings highlight that parent’s and children uniquely adjust their leading behaviors in synchrony in competition, presumably adjusting their behavior to accommodate a complex situation. Given the importance of cooperative and competitive interactions to overall social well-being, and the parent’s role of modeling behaviors for their child, findings may direct future guidance and treatment plans that will promote social development.

Từ khóa


Tài liệu tham khảo

Abraham, E. K., McCusker, M. E., & Foti, R. J. (2019). Competing conversations: An examination of competition as intrateam interactions. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 970.

Bell, D. C., & Richard, A. J. (2000). Caregiving: The forgotten element in attachment. Psychological Inquiry, 11(2), 69–83.

Bernieri, F. J., Davis, J. M., Rosenthal, R., & Knee, C. R. (1994). Interactional Synchrony and Rapport: Measuring Synchrony in Displays devoid of sound and facial affect. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20(3), 303–311. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167294203008.

Boker, S. M., Rotondo, J. L., Xu, M., & King, K. (2002). Windowed cross-correlation and peak picking for the analysis of variability in the association between behavioral time series. Psychological Methods, 7(3), 338–355. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.7.3.338.

Cirelli, L. K., Einarson, K. M., & Trainor, L. J. (2014). Interpersonal synchrony increases prosocial behavior in infants. Developmental Science, 17(6), 1003–1011. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12193.

Cui, X., Bryant, D. M., & Reiss, A. L. (2012). NIRS-based hyperscanning reveals increased interpersonal coherence in superior frontal cortex during cooperation. Neuroimage, 59(3), 2430–2437. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.003.

Daniels, A. M. (2007). Cooperation versus competition: Is there really such an issue? New Directions for Youth Development, 2007(115), 43–56. https://doi.org/10.1002/yd.222.

Davis, M., West, K., Bilms, J., Morelen, D., & Suveg, C. (2018). A systematic review of parent–child synchrony: It is more than skin deep. Developmental Psychobiology, 60(6), 674–691.

Eisenberg, J., & Thompson, W. F. (2011). The effects of Competition on improvisers’ motivation, stress, and creative performance. Creativity Research Journal, 23(2), 129–136. https://doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2011.571185.

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A. G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 1149–1160. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149.

Feldman, R. (2007). Parent–infant synchrony: Biological foundations and developmental outcomes. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(6), 340–345.

Feldman, R. (2012). Parent-infant synchrony: A biobehavioral model of mutual influences in the formation of affiliative bonds. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 77(2), 42–51.

Feldman, R., & Eidelman, A. I. (2007). Maternal postpartum behavior and the emergence of infant–mother and infant–father synchrony in preterm and full-term infants: The role of neonatal vagal tone. Developmental Psychobiology, 49, 290–302.

Flinn, M. V., Geary, D. C., & Ward, C. V. (2005). Ecological dominance, social competition, and coalitionary arms races: Why humans evolved extraordinary intelligence. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26(1), 10–46.

Fonzi, A., Schneider, B. H., Tani, F., & Tomada, G. (1997). Predicting children’s friendship status from their dyadic Interaction in structured situations of potential conflict. Child Development, 68(3), 496–506. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1997.tb01954.x.

Fujiwara, K., Hoegen, R., Gratch, J., & Dunbar, N. E. (2022). Synchrony facilitates altruistic decision making for non-human avatars. Computers in Human Behavior, 128, 107079. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2021.107079.

Fülöp, M. (2022). Cooperation and Competition. In The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Social Development (pp. 555–572). https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119679028.ch30.

Harrist, A. W., & Waugh, R. M. (2002). Dyadic synchrony: Its structure and function in children’s development. Developmental Review, 22(4), 555–592.

Henrich, J., & Muthukrishna, M. (2021). The origins and psychology of human cooperation. Annual Review of Psychology, 72(1), 207–240. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-081920-042106.

Hove, M. J., & Risen, J. L. (2009). It’s all in the timing: Interpersonal synchrony increases affiliation. Social Cognition, 27(6), 949–960.

Hurwitz, S. C. (2002). To be successful--let them play! (For Parents Particularly). Childhood Education, 79(2), 101+. https://link.gale.com/apps/doc/A96193637/AONE?u=anon~6783fa0e&sid=googleScholar&xid=d405cd24.

Keller, T., & Cacioppe, R. (2001). Leader‐follower attachments: Understanding parental images at work. Leadership & Organization Development Journal, 22(2), 70–75.

Kleinbub, J. R., & Ramseyer, F. T. (2021). rMEA: An R package to assess nonverbal synchronization in motion energy analysis time-series. Psychotherapy Research, 31(6), 817–830.

Kruppa, J. A., Reindl, V., Gerloff, C., Oberwelland Weiss, E., Prinz, J., Herpertz-Dahlmann, B., & Schulte-Rüther, M. (2021). Brain and motor synchrony in children and adolescents with ASD—a fNIRS hyperscanning study. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 16(1–2), 103–116.

Launay, J., Tarr, B., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2016). Synchrony as an adaptive mechanism for large-Scale Human Social Bonding. Ethology, 122(10), 779–789. https://doi.org/10.1111/eth.12528.

Liu, T., Saito, H., & Oi, M. (2015). Role of the right inferior frontal gyrus in turn-based cooperation and competition: A near-infrared spectroscopy study. Brain and Cognition, 99, 17–23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2015.07.001.

Lobel, A., Engels, R. C., Stone, L. L., & Granic, I. (2019). Gaining a competitive edge: Longitudinal associations between children’s competitive video game playing, conduct problems, peer relations, and prosocial behavior. Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 8(1), 76–87.

Lomas, D., Koedinger, K., Patel, N., Shodhan, S., Poonwala, N., & Forlizzi, J. L. (2017). Is difficulty overrated? The effects of choice, novelty and suspense on intrinsic motivation in educational games. Proceedings of the 2017 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems Denver Colorado USA. https://doi.org/10.1145/3025453.3025638.

Loulis, S., & Kuczynski, L. (1997). Beyond one hand clapping: Seeing bidirectionality in parent-child relations. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 14(4), 441–461.

Lunkenheimer, E. S., Olson, S. L., Hollenstein, T., Sameroff, A. J., & Winter, C. (2011). Dyadic flexibility and positive affect in parent–child coregulation and the development of child behavior problems. Development and Psychopathology, 23(2), 577–591. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457941100006X.

Lunkenheimer, E., Hamby, C. M., Lobo, F. M., Cole, P. M., & Olson, S. L. (2020). The role of dynamic, dyadic parent–child processes in parental socialization of emotion. Developmental Psychology, 56, 566–577. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000808.

Markova, G., Nguyen, T., & Hoehl, S. (2019). Neurobehavioral interpersonal synchrony in early development: The role of interactional rhythms. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 2078.

McElhaney, K. B., Allen, J. P., Stephenson, J. C., & Hare, A. L. (2009). Attachment and autonomy during adolescence. Handbook of adolescent psychology: Individual bases of adolescent development (3rd ed., 1 vol., pp. 358–403). John Wiley & Sons, Inc. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470479193.adlpsy001012.

Miles, L. K., Griffiths, J. L., Richardson, M. J., & Macrae, C. N. (2010). Too late to coordinate: Contextual influences on behavioral synchrony. European Journal of Social Psychology, 40(1), 52–60.

Nguyen, T., Schleihauf, H., Kungl, M., Kayhan, E., Hoehl, S., & Vrtička, P. (2021). Interpersonal neural synchrony during father–child problem solving: An fNIRS hyperscanning study. Child Development, 92(4), e565–e580.

Nyman-Salonen, P., Kykyri, V. L., Tschacher, W., Muotka, J., Tourunen, A., Penttonen, M., & Seikkula, J. (2021). Nonverbal synchrony in couple therapy linked to clients’ well-being and the therapeutic alliance. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 718353.

Pan, Y., Dikker, S., Zhu, Y., Yang, C., Hu, Y., & Goldstein, P. (2022). Instructor-learner body coupling reflects instruction and learning. Npj Science of Learning, 7(1), 1–9.

Paxton, A., & Dale, R. (2013). Argument disrupts interpersonal synchrony. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(11), 2092–2102. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2013.853089.

Pereira, A. F., Smith, L. B., & Yu, C. (2008). Social coordination in toddler’s word learning: Interacting systems of perception and action. Connection Science, 20(2–3), 73–89.

Ramnani, N., & Miall, R. C. (2004). A system in the human brain for predicting the actions of others. Nature Neuroscience, 7(1), 85–90.

Ramseyer, F. T. (2020). Motion energy analysis (MEA): A primer on the assessment of motion from video. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 67(4), 536–549.

Reindl, V., Gerloff, C., Scharke, W., & Konrad, K. (2018). Brain-to-brain synchrony in parent-child dyads and the relationship with emotion regulation revealed by fNIRS-based hyperscanning. Neuroimage, 178, 493–502.

Reindl, V., Wass, S., Leong, V., Scharke, W., Wistuba, S., Wirth, C. L., & Gerloff, C. (2022). Multimodal hyperscanning reveals that synchrony of body and mind are distinct in mother-child dyads. NeuroImage, 251, 118982.

Richard, J. F., Fonzi, A., Tani, F., Tassi, F., Tomada, G., & Schneider, B. H. (2002). Cooperation and competition. In P. K. Smith, & C. H. Hart (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of childhood social development (pp. 515–532). Blackwell Publishers Ltd.

Sheridan, S., & Williams, P. (2006). Constructive competition in preschool. Journal of Early Childhood Research, 4(3), 291–310.

Singer, D. G., Golinkoff, R. M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2006). Play = Learning: How play motivates and enhances children’s cognitive and social-emotional growth. Oxford University Press.

Sullivan, P. J., Rickers, K., & Gammage, K. L. (2014). The effect of different phases of synchrony on pain threshold. Group Dynamics: Theory Research and Practice, 18(2), 122–128. https://doi.org/10.1037/gdn0000001.

Thai, S., Lockwood, P., Zhu, R., Li, Y., & He, J. C. (2019). The family ties that protect: Expanded-self comparisons in parent–child relationships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 36(3), 1041–1066.

Tschacher, W., Rees, G. M., & Ramseyer, F. (2014). Nonverbal synchrony and affect in dyadic interactions. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1323.

Valcea, S., Hamdani, M. R., Buckley, M. R., & Novicevic, M. M. (2011). Exploring the developmental potential of leader–follower interactions: A constructive-developmental approach. The Leadership Quarterly, 22(4), 604–615. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2011.05.003.

van Houtum, L. A., Wever, M. C., Janssen, L. H., van Schie, C. C., Will, G. J., Tollenaar, M. S., & Elzinga, B. M. (2021). Vicarious praise and pain: Parental neural responses to social feedback about their adolescent child. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 16(4), 406–417.

Völter, C., Oberländer, K., Mertens, S., & Ramseyer, F. T. (2022). Nonverbal synchrony in subjects with hearing impairment and their significant others [Original Research]. Frontiers in Psychology, 13, 964547. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.964547.

Vygotsky, L. S. (2016). Play and its role in the Mental Development of the child. International Research in Early Childhood Education, 7(2), 3–25.

Weiss, M. R., & Hayashi, C. T. (1995). All in the family: Parent-child influences in competitive youth gymnastics. Pediatric Exercise Science, 7(1), 36–48. https://doi.org/10.1123/pes.7.1.36.

Wilson, M., & Wilson, T. P. (2005). An oscillator model of the timing of turn-taking. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12(6), 957–968.

Wiltermuth, S. S., & Heath, C. (2009). Synchrony and cooperation. Psychological Science, 20(1), 1–5.

Yarmolovsky, J., Sabag, M., Lipschits, O., & Geva, R. (2023). Parents regulate arousal while sharing experiences with their child: A study of pupil diameter change responses [Original Research]. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 17, https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2023.1177687.