Fixed-duration shock treatment: Pre- and posttreatment stimulation, activity, and skin resistance as predictors of escape performance

Springer Science and Business Media LLC - Tập 23 Số 4 - Trang 329-344 - 1995
D. Chris Anderson1, Charles R. Crowell1, Neil R. Boyd1, Donald A. Hantula2
1Department of Psychology, University of Notre Dame, 114 Haggar Hall, Notre Dame, Indiana, 46556, USA
2Temple University Philadelphia, Pennsylvania USA

Tóm tắt

Từ khóa


Tài liệu tham khảo

Anderson, D. C., Cole, J., & McVaugh, W. (1968). Variations in unsignaled inescapable preshock as determinants of responses to punishment. Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology Monograph Supplement, 65 (3, Pt. 2).

Anderson, D. C., Crowell, C. R., Cunningham, C. L., & Lupo, J. V. (1979). Behavior during shock exposure as a determinant of subsequent interference with shuttle box escape—avoidance learning in the rat. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 5, 243–257.

Anderson, D. C., Crowell, C. R., Koehn, D., & Lupo, J. V. (1976). Different intensities of unsignalled inescapable shock as determinants of non-shock-motivated open field behavior: A resolution of disparate results. Physiology & Behavior, 17, 391–394.

Anderson, D. C., Elder, M., O’Brien, R., & Pearson, J. (1970). The long term proactive effect of preshock and ECS as determinants of subsequent responses to punishment. Physiology & Behavior, 5, 389–396.

Anisman, H., & Sklar, L. S. (1979). Catecholamine depletion in mice upon reexposure to stress: Mediation of the escape deficits produced by inescapable shock. Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology, 93, 610–625.

Balleine, B., & Job, R. F. S. (1991). Reconsideration of the role of competing responses in demonstrations of the interference effect (learned helplessness). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 17, 270–280.

Black, A. H. (1977). Comments on “Learned Helplessness: Theory and Evidence” by Maier and Seligman. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 106, 41–43.

Blanchard, R. J., & Blanchard, D. C. (1969). Crouching as an index of fear. Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology, 67, 370–375.

Bouton, M. E., & Bolles, R. C. (1980). Conditioned fear assessed by freezing and by the suppression of three different baselines. Animal Learning & Behavior, 8, 429–434.

Brookshire, K. H., Littman, R. A., & Stewart, C. N. (1961). Residua of shock-trauma in the white rat: A three-factor theory. Psychological Monographs: General & Applied, 75 (10, Whole No. 514).

Campbell, B. A., & Teghtsoonian, R. (1958). Electrical and behavioral effects of different types of shock stimuli on the rat. Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology, 51, 185–192.

Cook, M., Mineka, S., & Trumble, D. (1987). The role of response-produced and exteroceptive feedback in the attenuation of fear over the course of avoidance learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 13, 239–249.

Crowell, C. R., & Anderson, D. C. (1979). Shuttle interference effects in the rat depend upon activity during prior shock: A replication. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 14, 413–416.

Crowell, C. R., & Anderson, D. C. (1981). Influence of duration and number of inescapable shocks on intrashock activity and subsequent interference effects. Animal Learning & Behavior, 9, 28–37.

Crowell, C. R., & Frei, L. (1972). A circuit to permit an electronic drinkometer to operate during footshock. Behavior Research Methods & Instrumentation, 4, 193–194.

Denny, M. R. (1971). Relaxation theory and experiments. In F. R. Brush (Ed.), Aversive conditioning and learning (pp. 235–295). New York: Academic Press.

Fanselow, M. S., DeCola, J. P., & Young, S. L. (1993). Mechanisms responsible for reduced contextual conditioning with massed unsignaled unconditioned stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 19, 121–137.

Feldman, M. A. (1977). The effects of preexposure to a warning or a safety signal on the acquisition of a two-way avoidance response in rats. Animal Learning & Behavior, 5, 21–24.

Fowler, H., Kleiman, M. C., & Lysle, D. T. (1985). Factors affecting the acquisition and extinction of conditioned inhibition suggest a “slave” process. In R. R. Miller & N. E. Spear (Eds.), Information processing in animals: Conditioned inhibition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Glazer, H. I., & Weiss, J. M. (1976). Long-term interference effect: An alternative to “learned helplessness.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 2, 202–213.

Jackson, R. L., & Minor, T. R. (1988). Effects of signaling inescapable shock on subsequent escape learning: Implications for theories of coping and “learned helplessness.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 14, 390–400.

Lawry, J. A., Lupo, J. V., Overmier, J. B., Kochevar, J., Hollis, K. L., & Anderson, D. C (1978). Interference with avoidance behavior as a function of qualitative properties of inescapable shocks. Animal Learning & Behavior, 6, 147–154.

Levine, S., Madden, J., IV, Connor, R. L., Moskal, J. R., & Anderson, D. C. (1973). Physiological and behavioral effects of prior aversive stimulation (preshock) in the rat. Physiology & Behavior, 10, 467–471.

Levis, D. (1976). Learned helplessness: A reply and alternative S–R interpretation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 105, 47–65.

Lubow, R. E. (1973). Latent inhibition. Psychological Bulletin, 79, 398–407.

Lubow, R. E. (1989). Latent inhibition and conditioned attention theory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Madden, J., IV, Rollins, J., Anderson, D. C., Conner, R. L., & Levine, S. (1971). Preshock-produced intensification of passive avoidance responding and of elevation in corticosteroid level. Physiology & Behavior, 7, 733–736.

Maier, S. F. (1990). Role of fear in mediating shuttle escape learning deficit produced by inescapable shock. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 16, 137–149.

Maier, S. F., Albin, R. W., & Testa, T. J. (1973). Failure to learn to escape in rats previously exposed to inescapable shock depends on the nature of the escape response. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 85, 581–592.

Maier, S. F., Davies, S., Grau, J. W., Jackson, R. L., Morrison, D. H., Moye, T., Madden, J., IV, & Barchas, J. D. (1980). Opiate antagonists and long-term analgesic reaction induced by inescapable shock in rats. Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology, 94, 1172–1183.

Maier, S. F., Grahn, R. E., Kalman, B. A., Sutton, L. C., Wiertelak, E. P., & Watkins, L. R. (1993). The role of the amygdala and dorsal raphe nucleus in mediating the behavioral consequences of inescapable shock. Behavioral Neuroscience, 107, 377–388.

Maier, S. F., & Keith, J. R. (1987). Shock signals and the development of stress-induced analgesia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 13, 226–238.

Maier, S. F., & Minor, T. R. (1993). Dissociation of interference with the speed and accuracy of escape produced by inescapable shock. Behavioral Neuroscience, 107, 139–146.

Maxwell, S. E., & Delaney, H. D. (1990). Designing experiments and analyzing data. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.

McAllister, W. R., & McAllister, D. E. (1992). Fear determines the effectiveness of a feedback stimulus in aversively motivated instrumental learning. Learning & Motivation, 23, 99–115.

Merrill, H. K., & Anderson, D. C. (1970). Attenuation of a passive-avoidance response via reinforcing intracranial stimulation in rats. Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology, 73, 274–277.

Mineka, S., Cook, M., & Miller, S. (1984). Fear conditioned with escapable and inescapable shock: Effects of a feedback stimulus. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 10, 307–323.

Minor, T. R., Dess, N. K., & Overmier, J. B. (1991). Inverting the traditional view of “learned helplessness.” In M. R. Denny (Ed.), Fear, avoidance, and phobias: A fundamental analysis. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Minor, T. R., & LoLordo, V. M. (1984). Escape deficits following inescapable shock: The role of contextual odor. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 10, 168–181.

Minor, T. R., Trauner, M. S., Lee, C. Y., & Dess, N. K. (1990). Modeling the signal features of an escape response: Effects of cessation conditioning in the “learned helplessness” paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 16, 123–136.

Mowrer, O. H. (1960). Learning theory and behavior. New York: Wiley.

O’Brien, R. G., & Kaiser, M. K. (1985). MANOVA method for analyzing repeated measures designs: An extensive primer. Psychological Bulletin, 97, 316–333.

Overmier, J. B. (1968). Interference with avoidance behavior: Failure to avoid traumatic shock. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 78, 340–343.

Overmier, J. B. (1988). Psychological determinants of when Stressors stress. In D. Hellhammer, I. Florin, & H. Weiner (Eds.), Neurobiological approaches to human disease (pp. 236–259). Toronto: Hans Huber.

Overmier, J. B., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1967). Effects of inescapable shock on subsequent escape and avoidance learning. Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology, 63, 28–33.

Payne, R., Anderson, D. C., & Murcurio, J. (1970). Preshock-produced alterations in pain-elicited fighting. Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology, 71, 258–266.

Rescorla, R. A., & Holland, P. C. (1977). Association in Pavlovian conditioned inhibition. Learning & Motivation, 8, 429–447.

Rescorla, R. A., & Wagner, A. R. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and non-reinforcement. In A. H. Black & W. F. Prokasy (Eds.), Classical conditioning II (pp. 64–99). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

Rosellini, R. A., DeCola, J. P., & Warren, D. A. (1986). The effect of feedback stimuli on contextual fear depends upon the length of the minimum intertrial interval. Learning & Motivation, 17, 229–242.

Rosellini, R. A., Warren, D. A., & DeCola, J. P. (1987). Predictability and controllability: Differential effects upon contextual fear. Learning & Motivation, 18, 392–420.

Solomon, R. L. (1980). The opponent-process theory of acquired motivation. American Psychologist, 35, 691–712.

Solomon, R. L., & Corbit, J. D. (1974). An opponent—process theory of motivation: I. The temporal dynamics of affect. Psychological Review, 81, 119–145.

Torrez, J. T., & Anderson, D. C. (1993). A biobehavioral analysis of the shock-treatment, punishment-intensification effect: Feedback contingency, fear, and endorphins. Unpublished master’s thesis, University of Notre Dame.

Volpicelli, J. R., Ulm, R. R., & Altenor, A. (1984). Feedback during exposure to inescapable shocks and subsequent shock-escape performance. Learning & Motivation, 15, 274–286.

Walters, G. C., & Rogers, J. V. (1963). Aversive stimulation of the rat: Long-term effects on subsequent behavior. Science, 142, 70–71.

Weiss, J. M., Goodman, P. A., Lositi, B. G., Corrigan, S., Charry, J. M., & Bailey, W. H. (1981). Behavioral depression produced by an uncontrollable Stressor: Relationship to norepinephrine, dopamine, and serotonin levels in various regions of rat brain. Brain Research Review, 3, 167–175.

Weiss, J. M., Kriekhaus, E. E., & Conte, R. (1968). Effects of fear conditioning on subsequent avoidance behavior and movement. Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology, 65, 413–421.

Whitehouse, W. G., Bersh, P. J., Blustein, J. E., & Troise, J. R., III (1988). Signals for shock-free periods during chronic exposure to delayed-escape and inescapable shocks: Effects on later escape acquisition. Learning & Motivation, 19, 142–161.