Evidence for benefits of argumentation in a Mayan indigenous population
Tài liệu tham khảo
Baumeister, 2010, Conscious thought is for facilitating social and cultural interactions: How mental simulations serve the animal–culture interface, Psychological Review, 117, 945, 10.1037/a0019393
Becker, 1986, Reasons for the lack of argumentation and debate in the Far East, International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 10, 75, 10.1016/0147-1767(86)90035-0
Billig, 1996
Castelain, T., Bernard, S., Van der Henst, J. B., & Mercier, H. (in press). The influence of power and reason on young Maya children’s endorsement of testimony. Developmental Science.
Chouinard, 2007, Children's questions: A mechanism for cognitive development, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 1
Cole, 1971
Dasen, 1972, Cross-cultural Piagetian research: A summary, Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 3, 23, 10.1177/002202217200300102
Dehaene, 1999
Dehaene, 2009
Descartes, 1637
Diamond, 2012
Doise, 1984
Fontanari, 2014, Probabilistic cognition in two indigenous Mayan groups, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 11, 17075, 10.1073/pnas.1410583111
Gauvain, 2013, Children's questions in cross-cultural perspective a four-culture study, Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 44, 1148, 10.1177/0022022113485430
Gibbard, 1990
Gluckman, 1967
Gordon, 2004, Numerical cognition without words: Evidence from Amazonia, Science, 306, 496, 10.1126/science.1094492
Gurven, 2007, Longevity among hunter-gatherers: A cross-cultural examination, Population and Development Review, 33, 321, 10.1111/j.1728-4457.2007.00171.x
Hahn, 2007, The rationality of informal argumentation: A bayesian approach to reasoning fallacies, Psychological Review, 114, 704, 10.1037/0033-295X.114.3.704
Hart, 1995
Heath, 1983
Henrich, 2010, The weirdest people in the world, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 61, 10.1017/S0140525X0999152X
INE, 2011
Jamet, 2014, Global warming and the rise of the sea level: A study of intellectual development in preadolescents and adolescents from 11 to 15 years old, Studia pédagogiczne, 24, 361
Janis, 1982
Kahan, 2012, The polarizing impact of science literacy and numeracy on perceived climate change risks, Nature Climate Change, 2, 732, 10.1038/nclimate1547
Kahneman, 2003, A perspective on judgment and choice: Mapping bounded rationality, American Psychologist, 58, 697, 10.1037/0003-066X.58.9.697
Kim, 1999, Deviance or uniqueness, harmony or conformity? A cultural analysis, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 785, 10.1037/0022-3514.77.4.785
Koriat, 1980, Reasons for confidence, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory and Cognition, 6, 107
Laughlin, 2011
Lévi-Strauss, 1966, The scope of anthropology, Current Anthropology, 7, 112, 10.1086/200687
Lloyd, 1996
Luria, 1976
Medin, 2004, The native mind: Biological categorization and reasoning in development and across cultures, Psychological Review, 111, 960, 10.1037/0033-295X.111.4.960
Mellers, 2014, Psychological strategies for winning a geopolitical forecasting tournament, Psychological Science, 25, 1106, 10.1177/0956797614524255
Mercier, 2011, On the universality of argumentative reasoning, Journal of Cognition and Culture, 11, 85, 10.1163/156853711X568707
Mercier, 2011, Reasoning serves argumentation in children, Cognitive Development, 26, 177, 10.1016/j.cogdev.2010.12.001
Mercier, 2011, When experts argue: Explaining the best and the worst of reasoning, Argumentation, 25, 313, 10.1007/s10503-011-9222-y
Mercier, H. (in pressa). Confirmation (or myside) bias. In R. Pohl (Ed.), Cognitive Illusions (2nd Ed.). London: Psychology Press.
Mercier, H. (in pressb). Reasoning and argumentation. In V. A. Thompson & L. J. Ball (Eds.), International Handbook of Thinking & Reasoning. Hove: Psychology Press.
Mercier, 2015, The benefits of argumentation are cross-culturally robust: The case of Japan, Thinking & Reasoning, 1
Mercier, 2011, Why do humans reason? Arguments for an argumentative theory, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34, 57, 10.1017/S0140525X10000968
Moshman, 1998, Collaborative reasoning: Evidence for collective rationality, Thinking and Reasoning, 4, 231, 10.1080/135467898394148
Nakamura, 1964
Nickerson, 1998, Confirmation bias: A ubiquitous phenomena in many guises, Review of General Psychology, 2, 175, 10.1037/1089-2680.2.2.175
Oetzel, 2001, Face and facework in conflict: A cross-cultural comparison of China, Germany, Japan, and the United States, Communication Monographs, 68, 235, 10.1080/03637750128061
Peng, 1999, Culture, dialectics and reasoning about contradiction, American Psychologist, 54, 741, 10.1037/0003-066X.54.9.741
Perret-Clermont, 1980
Petty, 1998, Attitude change: Multiple roles for persuasion variables, 323
Piaget, 1928
Piaget, 1974
Pye, 1986, Quiché Mayan speech to children, Journal of Child Language, 13, 85, 10.1017/S0305000900000313
Rowe, 1999, The Delphi technique as a forecasting tool: Issues and analysis, International Journal of Forecasting, 15, 353, 10.1016/S0169-2070(99)00018-7
Schwartz, 2004
Scribner, 1977, Modes of thinking and ways of speaking: Culture and logic reconsidered, 483
Sears, 1980, Self-interest vs. symbolic politics in policy attitudes and presidential voting, American Political Science Review, 74, 670, 10.2307/1958149
Slavin, 1995, Vol. 2nd
Smith, 2009, Why peer discussion improves student performance on in-class concept questions, Science, 323, 122, 10.1126/science.1165919
Stanovich, 2004
Taber, 2006, Motivated skepticism in the evaluation of political beliefs, American Journal of Political Science, 50, 755, 10.1111/j.1540-5907.2006.00214.x
Tesser, 1975, Some effects of time and thought on attitude polarization, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 262, 10.1037/h0076292
Thompson, 2008, The social utility of feature creep, 181
Trouche, 2014, Arguments, more than confidence, explain the good performance of reasoning groups, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 1958, 10.1037/a0037099
UNDP, 2010
UNDP, 2010
UNDP, 2012
UNDP, 2014
UNFPA, 2014