Epistemic rationality: Skepticism toward unfounded beliefs requires sufficient cognitive ability and motivation to be rational
Tài liệu tham khảo
Aarnio, 2005, Paranormal beliefs, education, and thinking styles, Personality and Individual Differences, 39, 1227, 10.1016/j.paid.2005.04.009
Abalakina-Paap, 1999, Beliefs in conspiracies, Political Psychology, 20, 637, 10.1111/0162-895X.00160
Asser, 1998, Child fatalities from religion-motivated medical neglect, Pediatrics, 101, 625, 10.1542/peds.101.4.625
Ballarini, 2017, Reasons and the “motivated numeracy effect”, 1580
Barrett, 2000, Exploring the natural foundations of religion, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 29, 10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01419-9
Berinsky, 2012, Evaluating online labor markets for experimental research: Amazon.com's mechanical Turk, Political Analysis, 20, 351, 10.1093/pan/mpr057
Bialek, 2017, The cognitive reflection test is robust to multiple exposures, Behavior Research Methods, 1
Blackmore, 1985, Belief in the paranormal: Probability judgements, illusory control, and the ‘chance baseline shift’, British Journal of Psychology, 76, 459, 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1985.tb01969.x
Bruder, 2013, Measuring individual differences in generic beliefs in conspiracy theories across cultures: Conspiracy mentality questionnaire, Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 225, 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00225
Buhrmester, 2011, Amazon's mechanical turk: A new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data?, Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6, 3, 10.1177/1745691610393980
Cacioppo, 1984, The efficient assessment of need for cognition, Journal of Personality Assessment, 48, 306, 10.1207/s15327752jpa4803_13
Casler, 2013, Separate but equal? A comparison of participants and data gathered via Amazon's mechanical turk, social media, and face-to-face behavioral testing, Computers in Human Behavior, 29, 2156, 10.1016/j.chb.2013.05.009
Chaiken, 1989, Heuristic and systematic information processing within and beyond the persuasion context, 212
Darwin, 2011, Belief in conspiracy theories: The role of paranormal belief, paranoid ideation and schizotypy, Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 1289, 10.1016/j.paid.2011.02.027
Department of Health and Human Services, 2010, Administration for children and families
Douglas, 2016, Someone is pulling the strings: Hypersensitive agency detection and belief in conspiracy theories, Thinking and Reasoning, 22, 57, 10.1080/13546783.2015.1051586
Flynn, 1987, Massive IQ gains in 14 nations: What IQ tests really measure, Psychological Bulletin, 101, 171, 10.1037/0033-2909.101.2.171
Frederick, 2005, Cognitive reflection and decision- making, Journal of Economic Perspectives, 19, 25, 10.1257/089533005775196732
Gervais, 2012, Analytic thinking promotes religious disbelief, Science, 336, 493, 10.1126/science.1215647
Gibbs, 2007, Moral judgment development across cultures: Revisiting Kohlberg's universality claims, Developmental Review, 443, 10.1016/j.dr.2007.04.001
Goertzel, 1994, Belief in conspiracy theories, Political Psychology, 15, 731, 10.2307/3791630
Goodman, 2012, Data collection in a flat world: The strengths and weaknesses of mechanical turk samples, Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 26, 213, 10.1002/bdm.1753
Graham, 2011, Mapping the moral domain, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101, 366, 10.1037/a0021847
Grebe, 2012, AIDS conspiracy beliefs and unsafe sex in Cape Town, AIDS and Behavior, 16, 761, 10.1007/s10461-011-9958-2
Hergovich, 2005, Critical thinking ability and belief in the paranormal, Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 1805, 10.1016/j.paid.2004.11.008
Hogg, 2010, Religion in the face of uncertainty: An uncertainty-identity theory account of religiousness, Personality and Social Psychology Review, 14, 72, 10.1177/1088868309349692
Huang, 1998, Trends in Black-White Test-Score Differentials: The WORDSUM vocabulary test, 303
Jolley, 2014, The social consequences of conspiracism: Exposure to conspiracy theories decreases intentions to engage in politics and to reduce one's carbon footprints, British Journal of Psychology, 105, 35, 10.1111/bjop.12018
Kahan, 2017, Rumors of the ‘nonreplication’ of the ‘motivated numeracy effect’ are greatly exaggerated
Kahan, 2017, Motivated numeracy and enlightened self-government, 1, 54
Kay, 2010, Religious belief as compensatory control, Personality and Social Psychology Review, 14, 37, 10.1177/1088868309353750
Kunda, 1990, The case for motivated reasoning, Psychological Bulletin, 108, 480, 10.1037/0033-2909.108.3.480
Lewandowsky, 2013, NASA faked the moon landing—Therefore (climate) science is a hoax: An anatomy of the motivated rejection of science, Psychological Science, 24, 622, 10.1177/0956797612457686
Lewis, 2011, The relationship between intelligence and multiple domains of religious belief: Evidence from a large adult US sample, Intelligence, 39, 468, 10.1016/j.intell.2011.08.002
Lipkus, 2001, General performance on a numeracy scale among highly educated samples, Medical Decision Making, 21, 37, 10.1177/0272989X0102100105
Lobato, 2014, Examining the relationship between conspiracy theories, paranormal beliefs, and pseudoscience acceptance among a university population, Applied Cognitive Psychology, 28, 617, 10.1002/acp.3042
Lynn, 2009, Average intelligence predicts religiosity across 137 countries, Intelligence, 37, 11, 10.1016/j.intell.2008.03.004
Malhotra, 2007
Morgan, 2010, Moral and religious convictions and intentions to vote in the 2008 presidential election, Analyses of Social Issues and Public Policy, 10, 307, 10.1111/j.1530-2415.2010.01204.x
Musch, 2002, Probability misjudgment, cognitive ability, and belief in the paranormal, British Journal of Psychology, 93, 169, 10.1348/000712602162517
Nahin, 2009, Costs of complementary and alternative medicine (CAM) and frequency of visits to CAM practitioners: United States, 2007, National Health Statistics Reports, 18, 1
Newheiser, 2011, The functional nature of conspiracy beliefs: Examining the underpinnings of belief in the Da Vinci Code conspiracy, Personality and Individual Differences, 51, 1007, 10.1016/j.paid.2011.08.011
Nichols, 2003, Are children moral objectivists? Children's judgments about moral and response-dependent properties, Cognition, 90, B23, 10.1016/S0010-0277(03)00160-4
Nickerson, 1998, Confirmation bias: A ubiquitous phenomenon in many guises, Review of General Psychology, 2, 175, 10.1037/1089-2680.2.2.175
te Nijenhuis, 2013, Is the Flynn effect on g?: A meta-analysis, Intelligence, 41, 802, 10.1016/j.intell.2013.03.001
Norenzayan, 2013, The origins of religious disbelief, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17, 20, 10.1016/j.tics.2012.11.006
Oliver, 2016, Rise of the Trumpenvolk: Populism in the 2016 election, The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 667, 189, 10.1177/0002716216662639
Oliver, 2014, Medical conspiracy theories and health behaviors in the United States, JAMA Internal Medicine, 174, 817, 10.1001/jamainternmed.2014.190
Orenstein, 2002, Religion and paranormal belief, Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 41, 301, 10.1111/1468-5906.00118
Pennycook, 2012, Analytic cognitive style predicts religious and paranormal belief, Cognition, 123, 335, 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.03.003
Pennycook, 2013, Cognitive style and religiosity, Memory & Cognition, 42, 1, 10.3758/s13421-013-0340-7
Pennycook, 2014, The role of analytic thinking in moral judgements and values, Thinking and Reasoning, 20, 188, 10.1080/13546783.2013.865000
Pennycook, 2015, On the reception and detection of pseudo-profound bullshit, Judgment and Decision making, 10, 549, 10.1017/S1930297500006999
Petty, 1986, The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion, Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 19, 123, 10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60214-2
Reeve, 2009, Expanding the g-nexus: Further evidence regarding the relations among national IQ, religiosity, and national health outcomes, Intelligence, 37, 495, 10.1016/j.intell.2009.06.003
Schwartz, 1997, The role of numeracy in understanding the benefit of screening mammography, Annals of Internal Medicine, 127, 966, 10.7326/0003-4819-127-11-199712010-00003
Shenhav, 2011, Divine intuition: Cognitive style influences belief in god, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
Shermer, 2011
Skitka, 2014, The psychological foundations of moral conviction
Skitka, 2008, Moral conviction and political engagement, Political Psychology, 29, 29, 10.1111/j.1467-9221.2007.00611.x
Ståhl, 2016, Moralized rationality: Relying on logic and evidence in the formation and evaluation of belief can be seen as a moral issue, PLoS One, 11, 10.1371/journal.pone.0166332
Stanovich, 1999
Stanovich, 2000, Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 645, 10.1017/S0140525X00003435
Stanovich, 2008, On the relative independence of thinking biases and cognitive ability, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 672, 10.1037/0022-3514.94.4.672
Swami, 2012, Examining conspiracist beliefs about the disappearance of Amelia Earhart, The Journal of General Psychology, 139, 244, 10.1080/00221309.2012.697932
Swami, 2010, Unanswered questions: A preliminary investigation of personality and individual difference predictors of 9/11 conspiracist beliefs, Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24, 749, 10.1002/acp.1583
Swami, 2011, Conspiracist ideation in Britain and Austria: Evidence of a monological belief system and associations between individual psychological differences and real-world and fictitious conspiracy theories, British Journal of Psychology, 102, 443, 10.1111/j.2044-8295.2010.02004.x
Swami, 2014, Analytic thinking reduces belief in conspiracy theories, Cognition, 133, 572, 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.08.006
Szaszi, 2017, The cognitive reflection test revisited: Exploring the ways individuals solve the test, Thinking and Reasoning, 23, 207, 10.1080/13546783.2017.1292954
Thomson, 2016, Investigating an alternate form of the cognitive reflection test, Judgment and Decision making, 11, 99, 10.1017/S1930297500007622
Turiel, 1978, Social regulations and domains of social concepts, Vol. 1, 45
Uscinski, 2014
Van der Linden, 2015, How come some people believe in the paranormal? Those who favor Bigfoot, UFOs and ghosts share a thinking style, Scientific American
Van Prooijen, 2017, Why education predicts decreased belief in conspiracy theories, Applied Cognitive Psychology, 31, 50, 10.1002/acp.3301
Van Prooijen, 2015, The influence of control on belief in conspiracy theories: Conceptual and applied extensions, Applied Cognitive Psychology, 29, 753, 10.1002/acp.3161
Van Prooijen, 2013, Belief in conspiracy theories: The influence of uncertainty and perceived morality, European Journal of Social Psychology, 43, 109, 10.1002/ejsp.1922
Van Prooijen, J.-W., Douglas, K., & De Inocencio, C. (in press). Connecting the dots: Illusory pattern perception predicts belief in conspiracies and the supernatural. European Journal of Social Psychology.
Van Prooijen, 2015, Political extremism predicts belief in conspiracy theories, Social Psychological and Personality Science, 6, 570, 10.1177/1948550614567356
Van Zomeren, 2012, On conviction's collective consequences: Integrating moral conviction with the social identity model of collective action, British Journal of Social Psychology, 51, 52, 10.1111/j.2044-8309.2010.02000.x
Whitson, 2008, Lacking control increases illusory pattern perception, Science, 322, 115, 10.1126/science.1159845
Wood, 2012, Dead and alive: Beliefs in contradictory conspiracy theories, Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3, 767, 10.1177/1948550611434786
Zaal, 2011, By any means necessary: The effects of regulatory focus and moral conviction on hostile and benevolent forms of collective action, British Journal of Social Psychology, 50, 670, 10.1111/j.2044-8309.2011.02069.x
Zaal, 2017, You're either with us or against us: Moral conviction determines how the politicized distinguish friend from foe, Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 20, 519, 10.1177/1368430215615682