Domain-general abilities applied to domain-specific tasks: Sensitivity to probabilities in perception, cognition, and language
Tài liệu tham khảo
Alloy, 1984, Assessment of covariation by humans and animals: The joint influence of prior expectations and current situational information, Psychological Review, 91, 112, 10.1037/0033-295X.91.1.112
Armstrong, 1983, What some concepts might not be, Cognition, 13, 263, 10.1016/0010-0277(83)90012-4
Attneave, 1953, Psychological probability as a function of experienced frequency, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46, 81, 10.1037/h0057955
Baron, 1988
Best, 1981, Perceptual equivalence of acoustic cues in speech and nonspeech perception, Perception and Psychophysics, 29, 191, 10.3758/BF03207286
Blumstein, 1979, Acoustic invariance for place of articulation in speech production: Evidence from measurements of the spectral characteristics of stop consonants, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 66, 1001, 10.1121/1.383319
Blumstein, 1980, Perceptual invariance and onset spectra for stop consonants in different vowel environments, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 67, 648, 10.1121/1.383890
Brown, 1957, Linguistic determinism and part of speech, Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 49, 454, 10.1037/h0057814
Bruno, 1988, Minimodularity and the perception of layout, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117, 161, 10.1037/0096-3445.117.2.161
Chi, 1978, Knowledge structures and memory development, 73
Clark, 1979, When nouns surface as verbs, Language, 55, 767, 10.2307/412745
Coren, 1977, Fifty centuries of right-handedness: The historical record, Science, 198, 631, 10.1126/science.335510
Cutler, 1992, Rhythmic cues to speech segmentation: Evidence from juncture misperception, Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 218, 10.1016/0749-596X(92)90012-M
Cutler, 1987, The predominance of strong initial syllables in the English vocabulary, Computer Speech and Language, 2, 133, 10.1016/0885-2308(87)90004-0
Cutler, 1988, The role of strong syllables in segmentation for lexical access, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14, 113
Cutler, 1987, A note on the role of phonological expectation in speech segmentation, Journal of Memory and Language, 26, 480, 10.1016/0749-596X(87)90103-3
Cutting, 1984, Three greadients and the perception of flat and curved surfaces, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 198, 10.1037/0096-3445.113.2.198
Davis, 1992
Estes, 1985, Some common aspects of models for learning and memory in lower animals and man, 151
Ferreira, 1986, The independence of syntactic processing, Journal of Memory and Language, 25, 348, 10.1016/0749-596X(86)90006-9
Ferreira, 1990, The use of verb information in syntactic parsing: A comparison of evidence from eye movements and word-by word self-paced reading, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 555
Fisher, 1991, On the semantic content of subcategorization frames, Cognitive Psychology, 23, 331, 10.1016/0010-0285(91)90013-E
Francis, 1982
Frazier, 1978, The sausage machine: A new parsing model, Cognition, 6, 291, 10.1016/0010-0277(78)90002-1
Frazier, 1982, Making and correcting errors during sentence comprehension: Eye movements in the analysis of structurally ambiguous sentences, Cognitive Psychology, 14, 178, 10.1016/0010-0285(82)90008-1
Frazier, 1987, Resolution of syntactic category ambiguities: Eye movements in parsing lexically ambiguous sentences, Journal of Memory and Language, 26, 505, 10.1016/0749-596X(87)90137-9
Gallistel, 1990
Gernsbacher, 1984, Resolving 20 years of inconsistent interactions between lexical familiarity and orthography, concreteness, and polysemy, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 256, 10.1037/0096-3445.113.2.256
Gleitman, 1988, Where learning begins: Initial representations for language learning, Vol. 3, 150
Gluck, 1988, From conditioning to category learning: An adaptive network model, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117, 227, 10.1037/0096-3445.117.3.227
Gould, 1991
Gropen, 1989, The learnability and acquisition of the dative alternation in English, Language, 65, 203, 10.2307/415332
Hasher, 1984, Automatic processing of fundamental information: The case of frequency of occurrence, American Psychologist, 39, 1372, 10.1037/0003-066X.39.12.1372
Hintzman, 1988, Judgments of frequency and recognition memory in a multiple-trace memory model, Psychological Review, 95, 528, 10.1037/0033-295X.95.4.528
Johnston, 1978, A test of the sophisticated guessing theory of word perception, Cognition, 10, 123
Jonides, 1992, Direct coding of frequency of occurrence, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 368
Just, 1992, A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memory, Psychological Review, 99, 122, 10.1037/0033-295X.99.1.122
Keil, 1989
Kelly, 1988, Rhythmic alternation and lexical stress differences in English, Cognition, 30, 107, 10.1016/0010-0277(88)90037-6
Kelly, 1988, Phonological biases in grammatical category shifts, Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 343, 10.1016/0749-596X(88)90060-5
Kelly, 1989, Rhythm and language change in English, Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 690, 10.1016/0749-596X(89)90004-1
Kelly, 1992, Using sound to solve syntactic problems: The role of phonology in grammatical category assignments, Psychological Review, 99, 349, 10.1037/0033-295X.99.2.349
Kelly, 1993
Kimball, 1973, Seven principles of surface structure parsing in natural language, Cognition, 2, 15, 10.1016/0010-0277(72)90028-5
Levy, 1983, It's frogs all the way down, Cognition, 15, 75, 10.1016/0010-0277(83)90034-3
Liberman, 1991
Lichenstein, 1978, Judged frequency of lethal events, Journal of experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 4, 551
Lieberman, 1990
Lisker, 1978, Rapid vs. rabid: A catalogue of acoustic features that may cue the distinction, Haskins Laboratories Status Report on Speech Research, SR-54, 127
Maclay, 1959, Hesitation phenomena in spontaneous English speech, Word, 15, 19, 10.1080/00437956.1959.11659682
MacWhinney, 1978, The acquisition of morphonology, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 43, 174, 10.2307/1166047
Massaro, 1991, Language processing and information integration, 259
McCloskey, 1983, Intuitive physics, Scientific American, 24, 122, 10.1038/scientificamerican0483-122
Miller, 1983, Studies on the categorization of speech by infants, Cognition, 13, 135, 10.1016/0010-0277(83)90020-3
Miller, 1979, Some effects of later-occurring information on the perception of stop consonant and semivowel, Perception and Psychophysics, 25, 457, 10.3758/BF03213823
Nisbett, 1980
Pang, 1992, Expectancy and stimulus frequency: A comparative analysis in rats and humans, Perception and Psychophysics, 51, 607, 10.3758/BF03211657
Pinker, 1989
Popova, 1973, Grammatical elements of language in the speech of preschool children, 269
Reicher, 1969, Perceptual recognition as a function of meaningfulness of stimulus material, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 275, 10.1037/h0027768
Repp, 1979, Relative amplitude of aspiration noise as a voicing cue for syllable-initial stop consonants, Language and Speed, 22, 173, 10.1177/002383097902200207
Repp, 1982, Phonetic trading relations and context effects: New experimental evidence for a speech mode of perception, Psychological Bulletin, 92, 81, 10.1037/0033-2909.92.1.81
Repp, 1987, Phonetic category boundaries are flexible, 89
Rescorla, 1968, Probability of shock in the presence and absence of CS in fear conditioning, Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 66, 1, 10.1037/h0025984
Rescorla, 1969, Conditioned inhibition of fear
Rescorla, 1988, Pavlovian conditioning: It's not what you think it is, American Psychologist, 43, 151, 10.1037/0003-066X.43.3.151
Rescorla, 1972, A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement, 64
Rosch, 1975, Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories, Cognitive Psychology, 7, 573, 10.1016/0010-0285(75)90024-9
Rubin, 1974, The subjective estimation of syllable frequency, Perception and Psychophysics, 16, 193, 10.3758/BF03203273
Ruke-Dravina, 1973, On the emergence of inflection in child language: A contribution based on Latvian speech data, 252
Saegert, 1973, Exposure, context, and interpersonal attraction, Journal of personality and Social Psychology, 25, 234, 10.1037/h0033965
Seligman, 1970, On the generality of the laws of learning, Psychological Review, 77, 406, 10.1037/h0029790
Shannon, 1976, Aristotelianism, Newtonianism, and the physics of the layman, Perception, 5, 241, 10.1068/p050241
Shapiro, 1969, The subjective estimation of relative word frequency, Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13, 638
Shapiro, 1993, Preferences for a verb's complements and their use in sentence processing, Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 96, 10.1006/jmla.1993.1006
Shedler, 1985, Availability: Plausible but questionable
Smith, 1981
Trueswell, J.C., M.K. Tanenhaus and S.M. Garnsey, in press a. Semantic influences on parsing: Use of thematic role information in syntactic ambiguity resolution. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition.
Trueswell, J.C., M.K. Tanenhaus and C. Kello, in press b. Verb-specific constraints in sentence processing: Separating effects of lexical preference from garden-paths. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition.
Underwood, 1983
Walley, 1983, Onset spectra and formant transitions in the adult's and child's perception of place of articulation in stop consonants, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 73, 1011, 10.1121/1.389149
Wasserman, 1984, Judging response-outcome relations: The role of response-outcome contingency, outcome probability, and method of information presentation, Memory and Cognition, 12, 270, 10.3758/BF03197676
Zajonc, 1968, Attitudinal effects of mere exposure, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Monograph Supplement, 9, 1, 10.1037/h0025848