Do humans make good decisions?
Tài liệu tham khảo
Wald, 1949, Bayes solutions of sequential decision problems, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A., 35, 99, 10.1073/pnas.35.2.99
Gold, 2001, Neural computations that underlie decisions about sensory stimuli, Trends Cogn. Sci., 5, 10, 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01567-9
Yang, 2007, Probabilistic reasoning by neurons, Nature, 447, 1075, 10.1038/nature05852
Gould, 2012, Effects of decision variables and intraparietal stimulation on sensorimotor oscillatory activity in the human brain, J. Neurosci., 32, 13805, 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2200-12.2012
Ernst, 2002, Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion, Nature, 415, 429, 10.1038/415429a
Kording, 2004, Bayesian integration in sensorimotor learning, Nature, 427, 244, 10.1038/nature02169
O’Reilly, 2013, Brain systems for probabilistic and dynamic prediction: computational specificity and integration, PLoS Biol., 11, e1001662, 10.1371/journal.pbio.1001662
Kahneman, 1982
Tversky, 1974, Judgment under uncertainty: heuristics and biases, Science, 185, 1124, 10.1126/science.185.4157.1124
Stewart, 2003, Prospect relativity: how choice options influence decision under risk, J. Exp. Psychol. Gen., 132, 23, 10.1037/0096-3445.132.1.23
Tsetsos, 2010, Preference reversal in multiattribute choice, Psychol. Rev., 117, 1275, 10.1037/a0020580
Louie, 2012, Efficient coding and the neural representation of value, Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci., 1251, 13, 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2012.06496.x
Stewart, 2006, Decision by sampling, Cogn. Psychol., 53, 1, 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2005.10.003
Louie, 2013, Normalization is a general neural mechanism for context-dependent decision making, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A., 110, 6139, 10.1073/pnas.1217854110
Vlaev, 2011, Does the brain calculate value?, Trends Cogn. Sci., 15, 546, 10.1016/j.tics.2011.09.008
Soltani, 2012, A range-normalization model of context-dependent choice: a new model and evidence, PLoS Comput. Biol., 8, e1002607, 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002607
Chau, 2014, A neural mechanism underlying failure of optimal choice with multiple alternatives, Nat. Neurosci., 17, 463, 10.1038/nn.3649
Rangel, 2012, Value normalization in decision making: theory and evidence, Curr. Opin. Neurobiol., 22, 970, 10.1016/j.conb.2012.07.011
Tremblay, 1999, Relative reward preference in primate orbitofrontal cortex, Nature, 398, 704, 10.1038/19525
Padoa-Schioppa, 2009, Range-adapting representation of economic value in the orbitofrontal cortex, J. Neurosci., 29, 14004, 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3751-09.2009
Sugrue, 2004, Matching behavior and the representation of value in the parietal cortex, Science, 304, 1782, 10.1126/science.1094765
Barlow, 1961, Possible principles underlying the transformation of sensory messages
Simoncelli, 2003, Vision and the statistics of the visual environment, Curr. Opin. Neurobiol., 13, 144, 10.1016/S0959-4388(03)00047-3
Fairhall, 2001, Efficiency and ambiguity in an adaptive neural code, Nature, 412, 787, 10.1038/35090500
Laughlin, 1981, A simple coding procedure enhances a neuron's information capacity, Z. Naturforsch. C, 36, 910, 10.1515/znc-1981-9-1040
Carandini, 2012, Normalization as a canonical neural computation, Nat. Rev. Neurosci., 13, 51, 10.1038/nrn3136
Bartlett, 1965, Dark and light adaptation
de Gardelle, 2011, Robust averaging during perceptual judgment, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A., 108, 13341, 10.1073/pnas.1104517108
Michael, 2013, Unreliable evidence: 2 sources of uncertainty during perceptual choice, Cereb. Cortex.
Feldman, 2009, The influence of categories on perception: explaining the perceptual magnet effect as optimal statistical inference, Psychol. Rev., 116, 752, 10.1037/a0017196
Appelle, 1972, Perception and discrimination as a function of stimulus orientation: the “oblique effect” in man and animals, Psychol. Bull., 78, 266, 10.1037/h0033117
Wei, 2012, Efficient coding provides a direct link between prior and likelihood in perceptual bayesian inference, Adv. Neural Infirm. Process. Syst., 25, 1313
Kable, 2009, The neurobiology of decision: consensus and controversy, Neuron, 63, 733, 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.09.003
Summerfield, 2011, Perceptual classification in a rapidly changing environment, Neuron, 71, 725, 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.06.022
Cheadle, 2014, Adaptive gain control during human perceptual choice, Neuron, 81, 1429, 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.01.020
Tsetsos, 2012, Using time-varying evidence to test models of decision dynamics: bounded diffusion vs. the leaky competing accumulator model, Front. Neurosci., 10.3389/fnins.2012.00079
Fischer, 2014, Serial dependence in visual perception, Nat. Neurosci., 17, 738, 10.1038/nn.3689
Jazayeri, 2007, A new perceptual illusion reveals mechanisms of sensory decoding, Nature, 446, 912, 10.1038/nature05739
Pouget, 2007, Paying attention to neurons with discriminating taste, Neuron, 53, 473, 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.02.004
Scolari, 2009, Adaptive allocation of attentional gain, J. Neurosci., 29, 11933, 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5642-08.2009
Aston-Jones, 2005, An integrative theory of locus coeruleus-norepinephrine function: adaptive gain and optimal performance, Annu. Rev. Neurosci., 28, 403, 10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.061604.135709
Michael, 2014, Priming by the variability of visual information, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A., 111, 7873, 10.1073/pnas.1308674111
Brenner, 2000, Adaptive rescaling maximizes information transmission, Neuron, 26, 695, 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)81205-2
Tsetsos, 2012, Salience driven value integration explains decision biases and preference reversal, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A., 109, 9659, 10.1073/pnas.1119569109
Pouget, 2001, The relevance of Fisher Information for theories of cortical computation and attention
Scolari, 2012, Optimal deployment of attentional gain during fine discriminations, J. Neurosci., 32, 7723, 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5558-11.2012
Shafir, 1993, Reason-based choice, Cognition, 49, 11, 10.1016/0010-0277(93)90034-S
Shafir, 1993, Choosing versus rejecting: why some options are both better and worse than others, Mem. Cognit., 21, 546, 10.3758/BF03197186
Gigerenzer, 2011, Heuristic decision making, Annu. Rev. Psychol., 62, 451, 10.1146/annurev-psych-120709-145346
Beck, 2012, Not noisy, just wrong: the role of suboptimal inference in behavioral variability, Neuron, 74, 30, 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.03.016
Yu, 2009, Sequential effects: superstition or rational behavior?, 1873
Glimcher, 2004
Stephens, 2008, Decision ecology: foraging and the ecology of animal decision making, Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci., 8, 475, 10.3758/CABN.8.4.475
Kolling, 2014, Multiple neural mechanisms of decision making and their competition under changing risk pressure, Neuron, 81, 1190, 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.01.033
Von Neumann, 2007
Allingham, 2002
Tversky, 1992, Advances in prospect theory: cumulative representation of uncertainty, J. Risk Uncertainty, 5, 297, 10.1007/BF00122574
Wyart, 2012, Rhythmic fluctuations in evidence accumulation during decision making in the human brain, Neuron, 76, 847, 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.015
Roitman, 2002, Response of neurons in the lateral intraparietal area during a combined visual discrimination reaction time task, J. Neurosci., 22, 9475, 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-21-09475.2002
Treue, 1999, Feature-based attention influences motion processing gain in macaque visual cortex, Nature, 399, 575, 10.1038/21176
Green, 1966
Wyart, 2012, Dissociable prior influences of signal probability and relevance on visual contrast sensitivity, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A., 109, 3593, 10.1073/pnas.1120118109
Kok, 2012, Less is more: expectation sharpens representations in the primary visual cortex, Neuron, 75, 265, 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.04.034